心理发展与教育 ›› 2018, Vol. 34 ›› Issue (3): 273-283.doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2018.03.03

• 认知与社会性发展 • 上一篇    下一篇

智能手机成瘾者的注意执行控制功能:心智游移的作用

林悦, 刘勤学, 邓寒, 李羽萱, 丁凯旋   

  1. 青少年网络心理与行为教育部重点实验室, 人的发展与心理健康湖北省重点实验室, 武汉, 430079;华中师范大学心理学院, 心理学国家级实验教学示范中心, 武汉, 430079
  • 出版日期:2018-05-15 发布日期:2018-05-15
  • 通讯作者: 刘勤学,E-mail:qinxueliu@mail.ccnu.edu.cn E-mail:qinxueliu@mail.ccnu.edu.cn
  • 基金资助:

    华中师范大学2017年基本科研业务费探索创新项目(CCNU17TS0008);湖北省教育科学规划2017年度课题(2017GB004);人的发展与心理健康湖北省重点实验室开放课题(20160402)。

Executive Control Function of Smart-phone Addicts: The Role of Mind-wandering

LIN Yue, LIU Qinxue, DENG Han, LI Yuxuan, DING Kaixuan   

  1. Key Laboratory of Adolescent Cyberpsychology and Behavior CCNU, Ministry of Education, Key Laboratory of Human Development and Mental Health of Hubei Province;Wuhan 430079;National Experimental Teaching Demonstrating Center of Psychology, School of Psychology, Central China Normal University, Wuhan 430079
  • Online:2018-05-15 Published:2018-05-15

摘要: 智能手机成瘾可能会影响个体的注意执行控制功能,以往研究结果在该影响是否取决于实验刺激与成瘾物的相关性上存在不一致,研究关注心智游移在其中的作用。采取《大学生智能手机成瘾量表》筛选被试90人分为成瘾组和非成瘾组。实验一采用反应的持续注意任务考察不同水平智能手机成瘾者的心智游移特点,采用《白日梦量表》辅助研究,结果发现智能手机成瘾被试量表得分显著更高,在实验中更多地评估任务表现,且其心智游移与偶然性的任务疏忽、一般性任务疏忽以及反应分离状态有关。实验二采用AX版本的持续操作任务探究智能手机成瘾者的注意执行控制功能中心智游移的调节作用,结果发现高频心智游移的智能手机成瘾者比低频心智游移的智能手机成瘾者维持目标更困难。研究对于理解智能手机成瘾对个体认知的影响具有一定的启示作用。

关键词: 大学生, 智能手机成瘾, 执行控制, 心智游移, 任务无关想法

Abstract: Damages have been found in smartphone addicts' executive control function, but how these damages happened and the potential factors are unclear, especially whether these damages depended on addictive clues. The present study aimed to explore the possible role of mind-wandering. 90 participants were screened according to the Smartphone Addiction Scale for College Students(SAS-C). In experiment 1, Sustained Attention to Response Task(SART) was selected, and the daydreaming scale in Imaginal Processes Inventory was used to investigate the mind-wandering frequency in their daily lives. The results showed that smartphone addicts had higher mind-wandering frequency in their daily lives, and they evaluated their performance more frequently on the task; and their mind wandering was on the state of occurrent task inattention, generic task inattention and response disengagement. Meanwhile the mind wandering of non-addicts was only on the state of occurrent task inattention. In experiment 2, AX Continuous Performance Task(AX-CPT) was selected to investigate different groups' executive control function further and in details. Results showed that smart phone addicts with high mind wandering frequency had difficulties in maintaining task goals.

Key words: college student, smartphone addiction, executive control, mind wandering, task-unrelated thought

Braver, T. S., Satpute, A. B., Rush, B. K., Racine, C. A., & Barch, D. M. (2005). Context processing and context maintenance in healthy aging and early stage dementia of the alzheimer's type. Psychology & Aging, 20(1), 33-46.

Chen, C. Y., Huang, M. F., Yen, J. Y., Chen, C. S., Liu, G. C., Yen, C. F., et al. (2015). Brain correlates of response inhibition in Internet gaming disorder. Psychiatry and clinical neurosciences, 69, 201-209.

Chen, J., Liang, Y., Mai, C., Zhong, X., & Qu, C. (2016). General deficit in inhibitory control of excessive smartphone users:Evidence from an event-related potential study. Frontiers in psychology, 7, 511-519.

Cheyne, J. A., Solman, G. J. F., Carriere, J. S. A., & Smilek, D. (2009). Anatomy of an error:A bidirectional state model of task engagement/disengagement and attention-related errors. Cognition, 111, 98-113.

Faust, M. E., Balota, D. A., Spieler, D. H., & Ferraro, F. R. (1999). Individual differences in information-processing rate and amount:implications for group differences in response latency. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 777-799.

Forster, S., & Lavie, N. (2014). Distracted by your mind? Individual differences in distractibility predict mind wandering. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40, 261-260.

Hadlington, L. J. (2015). Cognitive failures in daily life:Exploring the link with Internet addiction and problematic mobile phone use. Computers in Human Behavior, 51, 75-81.

Hu, N., He, S., & Xu, B. (2012). Different efficiencies of attentional orienting in different wandering minds. Consciousness & Cognition, 21, 139-148.

Hao, N., Wu, M., Runco, M. A., & Pina, J. (2015). More mind wandering, fewer original ideas:Be not distracted during creative idea generation. Acta psychologica, 161, 110-116.

Jeong, S. H., Kim, H. J., Yum, J. Y., & Hwang, Y. (2016). What type of content are smartphone users addicted to? SNS vs. games. Computers in Human Behavior, 54, 10-17.

Jeromin, F., Nyenhuis, N., & Barke, A. (2016). Attentional bias in excessive Internet gamers:Experimental investigations using an addiction Stroop and a visual probe. Journal of behavioral addictions, 5, 32-40.

Jeromin, F., Rief, W., & Barke, A. (2016). Using two web-based addiction Stroops to measure the attentional bias in adults with Internet Gaming Disorder. Journal of behavioral addictions, 5, 666-673.

Kwon, M., Kim, D. J., Cho, H., & Yang, S. (2013) The smartphone addiction scale:Development and validation of a short version for adolescents. PLoS ONE 8(12):e83558.

Lee, H., Ahn, H., Choi, S., & Choi, W. (2014). The SAMS:Smartphone addiction management system and verification. Journal of medical systems, 38(1), 1-10.

Lee, E. B. (2015). Too much information heavy smartphone and facebook utilization by African American young adults. Journal of Black Studies, 46, 44-61.

Lin, Y. H., Chang, L. R., Lee, Y. H., Tseng, H. W., Kuo, T. B., & Chen, S. H. (2014). Development and validation of the smartphone addiction inventory. PLoS one, 6 (9), e98312.

Liu, G. C., Yen, J. Y., Chen, C. Y., Yen, C. F., Chen, C. S., Lin, W. C., et al, C. H. (2014). Brain activation for response inhibition under gaming cue distraction in internet gaming disorder. The Kaohsiung journal of medical sciences, 30, 43-51.

Lorsbach, T. C., & Reimer, J. F. (2010). Developmental differences in cognitive control:Goal representation and maintenance during a continuous performance task. Journal of Cognition and Development, 11, 185-216.

Mason, M. F., Norton, M. I.., Van Horn, J. D., Wegner, D. M, Grafton, S.T., & Macrae, C. N. (2007). Wandering minds:The default network and stimulus-independent thought. Science, 315, 393-395.

McVay, J. C., & Kane, M. J. (2010) Does mind wandering reflect executive function or executive failure? Comment on Smallwood and Schooler (2006) and Watkins (2008). Psychological Bulletin, 136, 188-197.

Moisala, M., Salmela, V., Hietajärvi, L., Salo, E., Carlson, S., & Salonen, O., et al. (2016). Media multitasking is associated with distractibility and increased prefrontal activity in adolescents and young adults. Neuroimage, 134, 113-121.

Mok, J. Y., Choi, S. W., Kim, D. J., Choi, J. S., Lee, J., Ahn, H., et al. (2014). Latent class analysis on internet and smartphone addiction in college students. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, 10, 817-828.

Paxton, J. L., Barch, D. M., Racine, C. A., & Braver, T. S. (2008). Cognitive control, goal maintenance, and prefrontal function in healthy aging. Cerebral Cortex, 18, 1010-1028.

Richmond, L. L., Redick, T. S., & Braver, T. S. (2015). Remembering to prepare:The benefits (and costs) of high working memory capacity. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 41, 1764-1777.

Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2015). The science of mind wandering:empirically navigating the stream of consciousness. Annual review of psychology, 66, 487-518.

Sayette, M. A., Schooler, J. W., & Reichle, E. D. (2010). Out for a Smoke The Impact of Cigarette Craving on Zoning Out During Reading. Psychological science, 21, 26-30.

Sayette, M. A., Reichle, E. D., & Schooler, J. W. (2009). Lost in the sauce the effects of alcohol on mind wandering. Psychological Science, 20, 747-752.

Sarwar, M., & Soomro, T. R. (2013). Impact of smartphone's on society. European journal of scientific research, 98, 216-226.

Smallwood, J., McSpadden, M., & Schooler, J. W. (2007). The lights are on but no one's home:Meta-awareness and the decoupling of attention when the mind wanders. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 527-533.

Schooler, J. W., Smallwood, J., Christoff, K., Handy, T. C., Reichle, E. D., & Sayette, M. A. (2011). Meta-awareness, perceptual decoupling and the wandering mind. Trends in cognitive sciences, 15, 319-326.

Stothart, C., Mitchum, A., & Yehnert, C. (2015). The attentional cost of receiving a cell phone notification. Journal of experimental psychology:human perception and performance, 41, 893-897.

Thornton, B., Faires, A., Robbins, M., & Rollins, E. (2014). The mere presence of a cell phone may be distracting:implications for attention and task performance. Social Psychology, 45, 479-488.

Van der Schuur, W. A., Baumgartner, S. E., Sumter, S. R., & Valkenburg, P. M. (2015). The consequences of media multitasking for youth:A review. Computers in Human Behavior, 53, 204-215.

Ward, A. F., Duke, K., Gneezy, A., & Bos, M. W. (2017). Brain drain:The mere presence of one's own smartphone reduces available cognitive capacity. Journal of the Association for Consumer Research, 2, 140-154.

Wilmer, H. H., Sherman, L. E., & Chein, J. M. (2017). Smartphones and cognition:A review of research exploring the links between mobile technology habits and cognitive functioning. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 605-921.

Zedelius, C. M., Broadway, J. M., & Schooler, J. W. (2015). Motivating meta-awareness of mind wandering:A way to catch the mind in flight? Consciousness and cognition, 36, 44-53.

戴珅懿, 马庆国, 王小毅. (2011). 网络游戏成瘾者对成瘾相关线索的注意偏向:一项ERP研究. 心理科学, 34, 1302-1307.

胡楠荼. (2012).心智游移与注意网络的关系(博士学位论文), 浙江大学,杭州.

刘勤学, 杨燕, 林悦, 余思, 周宗奎. (2017). 智能手机成瘾:概念、测量及影响因素. 中国临床心理学杂志, 25, 82-87.

雷雳, 杨洋. (2007). 青少年病理性互联网使用量表的编制与验证. 心理学报, 39, 688-696.

宋晓兰, 王晓, 唐威. (2011). 心智游移:现象、机制及意义. 心理科学进展, 19, 499-509.

苏双, 刘勤学, 潘婷婷, 王宇静.(2014). 大学生智能手机成瘾与自我控制的关系.中国学校卫生. 35, 155-157

苏双, 潘婷婷, 刘勤学, 陈潇雯, 王宇静, 李明月. (2014). 大学生智能手机成瘾量表的初步编制. 中国心理卫生杂志,28, 392-397.

汪海彬, 陶炎坤, 徐宏图. (2015). 不同手机依赖程度大学生的抑制控制特点.中国心理卫生杂志, 29, 226-229.

王寅宜, 宋晓兰. (2011). 心智游移及其相关现象的测量.浙江外国语学院学报, (3),101-108.
[1] 郭嘉程, 董柔纯, 许放, 徐旋, 牛更枫, 周宗奎. 社会临场感与大学生网络过激行为的关系:双自我意识的并行中介及性别的调节作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(2): 176-186.
[2] 樊香麟, 崔英锦. 客体化身体意识与女大学生限制性饮食行为的关系:外貌负面评价恐惧和社会文化压力的中介作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(1): 122-131.
[3] 张慧如, 张伟达, 傅王倩, 邓敏, 彭苏浩, 李玉. 孤独感对创造性倾向的影响:无聊倾向和焦虑情绪的中介作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(1): 132-141.
[4] 何安明, 张钰睿, 惠秋平. 大学生感恩与社会幸福感的关系:手机冷落行为的中介作用和负性生活事件的调节作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(4): 505-512.
[5] 杜秀芳, 武玉玺, 徐政, 袁晓倩, 陈功香. 金钱启动与道德认同对大学生道德伪善的影响[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(3): 342-349.
[6] 喻昊雪, 李卉, 王福兴. 大学生公正世界信念与学业倦怠的关系:应对方式与无聊倾向的链式中介作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(3): 391-401.
[7] 曾子豪, 彭丽仪, 詹林, 刘双金, 欧阳晓优, 丁道群, 黎志华, 胡义秋, 方晓义. 儿童期受虐对大学生抑郁症状的影响:主观幸福感的中介和基因的调节作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(2): 276-285.
[8] 曹瑞琳, 梅松丽, 梁磊磊, 李传恩, 张莹. 感恩与大学生网络成瘾的关系:核心自我评价和生命意义感的中介作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(2): 286-294.
[9] 陈子循, 李金文, 王雨萌, 刘霞. 累积环境风险与大学生自伤的关系:情绪调节策略的作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(1): 109-120.
[10] 郑爽, 刘红瑞, 李静, 席雨, 姚梅林. 主动性人格与大学生创业准备行为的关系:创业意向的中介效应与创业社会支持的调节作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2022, 38(6): 813-821.
[11] 王浩, 俞国良. 大学生依恋焦虑与抑郁的关系:恋爱中关系攻击和关系质量的序列中介作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2022, 38(6): 879-885.
[12] 朱黎君, 杨强, 叶宝娟, 陈智楠, 张丽. 自然联结对大学生抑郁的影响:有调节的中介模型[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2022, 38(6): 886-893.
[13] 张宝生, 李鑫, 李新野, 张庆普. 主观规范对大学生志愿者志愿工作投入的影响机制研究——一个链式双重中介模型[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2022, 38(5): 658-666.
[14] 李松, 陈旭, 冉光明, 张琪. 被动性社交网站使用与社交焦虑:反刍思维和自我建构的作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2022, 38(5): 720-728.
[15] 蔺姝玮, 孙炳海, 黄嘉昕, 肖威龙, 李伟健. 共情对广义互惠的影响:自我-他人重叠的中介作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2022, 38(4): 475-484.
Viewed
Full text


Abstract

Cited

  Shared   
  Discussed   
No Suggested Reading articles found!