心理发展与教育 ›› 2019, Vol. 35 ›› Issue (1): 1-10.doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2019.01.01

• 认知与社会性发展 •    下一篇

3~6岁儿童执行功能子成分发展的研究

王静梅1,2, 张义宾3, 郑晨烨2,4, 卢英俊1,2, 秦金亮2   

  1. 1. 浙江师范大学儿童研究院, 杭州 310012;
    2. 浙江师范大学杭州幼儿师范学院, 杭州 310012;
    3. 华东师范大学教育学部学前教育系, 上海 200062;
    4. 浙江省级机关北山幼儿园, 杭州 310012
  • 发布日期:2019-01-30
  • 通讯作者: 卢英俊,E-mail:luyingjun@zjnu.cn E-mail:luyingjun@zjnu.cn
  • 基金资助:
    全国教育科学规划国家重点课题(AHA110004);浙江省社科规划一般课题(15JDET02YB)。

Study on the Development of Executive Function Subcomponents in 3-6-year-old Children

WANG Jingmei1,2, ZHANG Yibin3, ZHENG Chenye2,4, LU Yingjun1,2, QIN Jinliang2   

  1. 1. Institute of Child Study, Zhejiang Normal University, Hangzhou 310012;
    2. Hangzhou College of Early Childhood Teachers Education, Zhejiang Normal University, Hangzhou 310012;
    3. Faculty of Education, East China Normal University, Shanghai 200062;
    4. Zhejiang Beishan Kindergarten, Hangzhou 310012
  • Published:2019-01-30

摘要: 执行功能是对目标指向活动进行信息整合和控制调节的一系列认知能力,在学前期发展迅速。研究使用执行功能任务组软件考察了210名3~6岁儿童(6组,组间年龄跨度6个月)执行功能各子成分(工作记忆、抑制控制和认知灵活性)的发展特征。结果表明:(1)随年龄增长,3~6岁儿童执行功能呈上升趋势发展,4岁左右为抑制控制与认知灵活性的高速发展期;(2)执行功能各子成分发展不同步,抑制控制最快,工作记忆最慢;(3)各子成分间呈中度正相关。结论:3~6岁执行功能能力发展迅速,但各子成分的发展进程并不一致;幼儿阶段执行功能测试的年龄分组最好精确到6个月。

关键词: 执行功能, 学前儿童, 工作记忆, 抑制控制, 认知灵活性

Abstract: Executive Functions, which were rapidly developed at early age, refer to a set of cognitive abilities involved in the control and coordination of information in the service of goal-directed activities. By using a newly developed Executive Function Task Battery, 210 3-6-year-old Chinese children (6 groups, each 6-month interval) were asked to finish the tasks, which test the developmental changes of the subcomponents of Executive Function (Working Memory, Inhibitory Control and Cognitive Flexibility).The results showed that the EF ability were rapidly improved during the development, especially in Inhibitory Control and Cognitive Flexibility of 4-year-old preschoolers; but the EF ability was increased out of step, which Inhibitory Control came first and Working Memory came last. The positive moderate correlations were found between the subcomponents. In conclusion, EF ability improved significantly during the early age, but was not inconformity. We suggest the selection of age on children' EF ability study can be accurate to 6 months.

Key words: executive function, preschooler, working memory, inhibitory control, cognitive flexibility

中图分类号: 

  • B844
Barkley, R. A. (2001). The executive functions and self-regulation:An evolutionary neuropsychological perspective. Neuropsychology Review, 11(1), 1-29.
Bell, M. A., Wolfe, C. D., & Adkins, D. R. (2007). Development of the prefrontal lobe in infants and children. In D. Coch, K. W. Fischer & G. Dawson (Eds.), Human behavior, learning, and the developing brain:Typical development (pp. 247-276). New York:The Guilford Press.
Best, J. R., Miller, P. H., & Jones, L. L. (2009). Executive functions after age 5:Changes and correlates. Developmental Review, 29(3), 180-200.
Best, J. R., & Miller, P. H. (2010). A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Development, 81(6), 1641-1660.
Blair, C. (2002). School readiness:Integrating cognition and emotion in a neurobiological conceptualization of children's functioning at school entry. American psychologist, 57(2), 111-127.
Carlson, S. M., Moses, L. J., & Breton, C. (2002). How specific is the relation between executive function and theory of mind? Contributions of inhibitory control and working memory. Infant and Child Development, 11(2), 73-92.
Carlson, S. M. (2005). Developmentally sensitive measures of executive function in preschool children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28(2), 595-616.
Davidson, M. C., Amso, D., Anderson, L.C., & Diamond, A. (2006). Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4 to 13 years:Evidence from manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching. Neuropsychologia, 44(11), 2037-2078.
Diamond, A. (2002). Normal development of prefrontal cortex from birth to young adulthood:Cognitive functions, anatomy, and biochemistry. In D.T. Stuss & R.T. Knight (Eds.), Principles of frontal lobe function (pp. 466-503). New York:Oxford University Press.
Diamond, A. (2006). The early development of executive functions. In E. Bialystok & F. Craik (Eds.), Lifespan cognition:Mechanisms of change (pp. 70-95). New York:Oxford University Press.
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64(1), 135-168.
Diamond, A., & Taylor, C. (1996). Development of an aspect of executive control:Development of the abilities to remember what I said and to "Do as I say, not as I do". Developmental Psychobiology, 29(4), 315-334.
Diamond, A., Prevor, M. B., Callender, G., & Druin, D. P. (1997). Prefrontal cortex deficits in children treated early and continuously for PKU. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 62 (4), i-v, 1-206.
Diamond, A., Kirkham, N., & Amso, D. (2002). Conditions under which young children can hold two rules in mind and inhibit a prepotent response. Developmental Psychology, 38(3), 352-362.
Diamond, A., Carlson, S. M., & Beck, D. M. (2005). Preschool children's performance in task switching on the Dimensional Change Card Sort Task:Separating the dimensions aids the ability to switch. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28(2), 689-729.
Espy, K. A., Sheffield, T. D., Wiebe, S., Clark, C. A. C., & Moehr, M. (2011). Executive control and dimensions of problem behaviors in preschool children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52(1), 33-46.
Frye, D., Zelazo, P. D., & Palfai, T. (1995). Theory of mind and rule-based reasoning. Cognitive Development, 10(4), 483-527.
Garon, N., Bryson, S. E., & Smith, I. M. (2008). Executive function in preschoolers:A review using an integrative framework. Psychological Bulletin, 134(1), 31-60.
Gerstadt, C. L., Hong, Y., & Diamond, A. (1994). The relationship between cognition and action:Performance of children 3 1/2-7 years old on a Stroop-like day-night test. Cognition, 53(2), 129-153.
Hala, S., Hug, S., & Henderson, A. (2003). Executive function and false-belief understanding in preschool children:Two tasks are harder than one. Journal of cognition and development, 4(3), 275-298.
Hughes, C., Ensor, R., Wilson, A., & Graham, A. (2010). Tracking executive function across the transition to school:A latent variable approach. Developmental Neuropsychology, 35(1), 20-36.
Huizinga, M., Dolan, C. V., & van der Molen, M. W. (2006). Age-related change in executive function:Developmental trends and a latent variable analysis. Neuropsychologia, 44(11), 2017-2036.
Jacques, S., & Zelazo, P. D. (2001). The flexible item selection task (FIST):A measure of executive function in preschoolers. Developmental Neuropsychology, 20(3), 573-591.
Jonides, J., & Smith, E. E. (1997). The architecture of working memory. In M. D. Rugg (Ed.), Cognitive neuroscience (pp. 243-276). Cambridge, MA:MIT Press.
Lee, K., Bull, R., & Ho, R. M. H. (2013). Developmental changes in executive functioning. Child Development, 84(6), 1933-1953.
Lehto, J. (1996). Are executive function tests dependent on working memory capacity? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49A(1), 29-50.
Logue, S. F., & Gould, T. J. (2014). The neural and genetic basis of executive function:Attention, cognitive flexibility, and response inhibition. Pharmacology Biochemistry & Behavior,123, 45-54.
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex "frontal lobe" tasks:A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49-100.
Rabbitt, P. (1997). Introduction:Methodologies and models in the study of executive function. In P. Rabbitt (Ed.), Methodology of frontal and executive function (pp. 1-38). Hove, UK:Psychology Press.
Richmond, J. B., Fisher, P. A., & Bales, S. N. (2011). Building the brain's "air traffic control" system:how early experiences shape the development of executive function. working paper 11. National Scientific Council on the Developing Child, 20.
Rothbart, M. K., Posner, M. I., & Kieras, J. (2008). Temperament, attention and the development of self-regulation. In K. McCartney & D. Phillips (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of early childhood development (pp. 328-357). Malden, MA:Blackwell Press.
Rueda, M. R., Rothbart, M. K., McCandliss, B. D., Saccomanno, L., & Posner, M. I. (2005). Training, maturation, and genetic influences on the development of executive attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science of the United States of America, 102(41), 14931-14936.
Shing, Y. L., Lindenberger, U., Diamond, A., Li, S. C., & Davidson, M. C. (2010). Memory maintenance and inhibitory control differentiate from early childhood to adolescence. Developmental Neuropsychology, 35(6), 679-697.
Wiebe, S. A., Espy, K. A., & Charak, D. (2008). Using confirmatory factor analysis to understand executive control in preschool children:I. Latent structure. Developmental Psychology, 44(2), 575-587.
Wiebe, S. A., Sheffield, T., Nelson, J. M., Clark, C. A., Chevalier, N., & Espy, K. A. (2011). The structure of executive function in 3-year-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108(3), 436-452.
Wiebe, S. A., Sheffield, T. D., & Espy, K. A. (2012). Separating the fish from the sharks:A longitudinal study of preschool response inhibition. Child Development, 83(4), 1245-1261.
Willoughby, M. T., & Blair, C. B. (2011). Test-retest reliability of a new executive function battery for use in early childhood. Child Neuropsychology, 17(6), 564-579.
Willoughby, M. T., Blair, C. B., Wirth, R. J., Greenberg, M., & Family Life Project Investigators. (2010). The measurement of executive function at age 3 years:Psychometric properties and criterion validity of a new battery of tasks. Psychological Assessment, 22(2), 306-317.
Willoughby, M. T., Blair, C. B., Wirth, R. J., Greenberg, M., & Family Life Project Investigators. (2012a). The measurement of executive function at age 5:Psychometric properties and relationship to academic achievement. Psychological Assessment, 24(1), 226-239.
Willoughby, M. T., Wirth, R. J., Blair, C. B., & Family Life Project Investigators. (2012b). Executive function in early childhood:Longitudinal measurement invariance and developmental change. Psychological Assessment, 24(2), 418-431.
Willoughby, M. T., Pek, J., & Blair, C. B. (2013). Measuring executive function in early childhood:A focus on maximal reliability and the derivation of short forms. Psychological Assessment, 25(2), 664-670.
Willoughby, M. T., & Blair, C. B. (2016). Measuring executive function in early childhood:A case for formative measurement. Psychological Assessment, 28(3), 319-330.
Zelazo, P. D., & Müller, U. (2002). Executive function in typical and atypical development. In U. Goswami (Ed.), Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development (pp. 445-469). Oxford, UK:Blackwell.
Zelazo, P. D., Müller, U., & Frye, D. (2003). The development of executive function in early childhood. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 68(3), 1-137.
陈天勇, 李德明. (2005). 执行功能可分离性及年龄关系的潜变量分析. 心理学报, 37(2), 210-217.
崔金奇. (2008). 4~6岁幼儿执行功能性别差异性研究. 中国健康心理学杂志, 16(10), 1160-1162.
丁锦红, 郭春彦. (2001). 工作记忆的脑机制研究. 心理科学, 24(5), 583-585.
黄海雁. (2017). 3~5岁幼儿执行功能和气质对说谎行为的影响(硕士学位论文). 浙江理工大学, 杭州.
李红, 高山, 王乃弋. (2004). 执行功能研究方法评述. 心理科学进展, 12(5), 693-705.
李媛媛. (2010). 4~7岁儿童执行功能的发展及其与相关影响因素关系的研究(硕士学位论文). 辽宁师范大学, 沈阳.
栾文双, 王静梅, 卢英俊. (2013). 学前儿童执行功能研究综述. 幼儿教育:教育科学, 7, 81-85.
魏勇刚, 吴睿明, 李红. (2005). 抑制性控制在幼儿执行功能与心理理论中的作用. 心理学报, 35(5), 598-605.
谢谟. (2007). 3~5周岁儿童气质、抑制性控制与心理理论的关系研究(硕士学位论文). 福建师范大学, 福州.
张乾一, 文萍. (2013). 3岁幼儿抑制和工作记忆的可分离性研究. 心理发展与教育, 29(3), 238-246.
张文静, 徐芬. (2005). 3~5岁幼儿执行功能的发展. 应用心理学, 11(1), 73-78.
张婷. (2010). 执行功能和心理理论的关系——发展和神经机制研究(博士学位论文). 西南大学, 重庆.
[1] 冷欣怡, 苏萌萌, 李文玲, 杨秀杰, 邢爱玲, 张湘琳, 舒华. 家庭环境与农村儿童早期语言发展的关系[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(1): 8-18.
[2] 彭越, 张和颐, 陈英和, 雷秀雅, 戚玥, 于晓, 乔学文, 刘沫漩, 颜露懿. 类比推理策略与工作记忆、抑制控制关系的年龄差异[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(6): 761-771.
[3] 颜志强, 周可, 曾晓, 徐惠, 朱晓倩, 张娟. 学前期儿童执行功能与攻击性行为的关系:认知共情的中介作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(6): 788-797.
[4] 于晓, 张和颐, 戚玥, 陈英和, 刘浩宁, 赵煜燨, 乔学文. 抑制控制在类比映射发展中的作用:认知负荷的影响[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(1): 1-11.
[5] 刘钰, 苏彦捷, 曹春梅, 丁宇, 樊煜. 社区老年人执行功能和加工速度与运动能力的关系[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2022, 38(6): 769-777.
[6] 宋佳汝, 刘媛媛, 王秀礼, 李寿欣. 视觉与言语工作记忆表征对视觉注意的引导[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2022, 38(5): 609-617.
[7] 温凯玲, 陈萍, 杨双, 宁宁. 前摄干扰对汉语听写困难儿童字形工作记忆的影响[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2022, 38(2): 153-160.
[8] 吴国婧, 程雪林, 李叶, 白荣, 邢淑芬, 李玉华. 学前期儿童执行功能和语言的双向关系:社会经济地位的调节[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2022, 38(2): 186-194.
[9] 王颖, 朱海东, 张馨艺, 肖沿. 学龄前留守儿童亲子依恋对执行功能的影响:祖孙依恋的调节作用和中介作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2022, 38(2): 195-206.
[10] 许郡婷, 鲍未, 罗俊龙. 不稳定控制感剥夺损害工作记忆刷新功能[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2022, 38(1): 1-9.
[11] 白荣, 闫嵘, 王千, 李叶, 邢淑芬. 学前儿童执行功能与问题行为的关系:情境和性别的特异性[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2022, 38(1): 35-44.
[12] 任屹, 黄四林. 贫困损害儿童执行功能的作用机制[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2022, 38(1): 134-143.
[13] 王兴华, 洪慧芳, 朱瑞玲. 2~3岁幼儿执行功能与母子互动的关系:幼儿气质的调节作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2021, 37(6): 784-791.
[14] 张光珍, 梁淼, 梁宗保. 父母教养方式影响学前儿童社会适应的追踪研究:自我控制的中介作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2021, 37(6): 800-807.
[15] 占淑玮, 杨宁, 赵必华. 留守学前儿童接受性语言能力与社会退缩的关系:有调节的中介模型[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2021, 37(6): 834-844.
Viewed
Full text


Abstract

Cited

  Shared   
  Discussed   
No Suggested Reading articles found!