心理发展与教育 ›› 2018, Vol. 34 ›› Issue (5): 567-575.doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2018.05.07

• 教与学心理 • 上一篇    下一篇

不同类型反馈对儿童学习效果的影响及性别差异

邹雨晨1,2, 丁颖1,3, 张旭然1, 李燕芳1   

  1. 1. 北京师范大学中国基础教育质量监测协同创新中心, 北京 100875;
    2. 重庆市南开中学校, 重庆 400030;
    3. 北京师范大学认知神经科学与学习国家重点实验室, 北京 100875
  • 出版日期:2018-09-15 发布日期:2018-10-25
  • 通讯作者: 李燕芳,E-mail:liyanfang@bnu.edu.cn E-mail:liyanfang@bnu.edu.cn

The Effects of Feedback Type on Children's Learning Performance and the Gender Differences in These Effects

ZOU Yuchen1,2, DING Ying1,3, ZHANG Xuran1, LI Yanfang1   

  1. 1. Collaborative Innovation Center of Assessment Toward Basic Education Quality, Beijing Normal University, Beijing 100875, China;
    2. Chongqing Nankai Middle School, Chongqing 400030, China;
    3. National Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning, Beijing Normal University, Beijing 100875, China
  • Online:2018-09-15 Published:2018-10-25

摘要: 儿童阶段是对外部反馈最敏感的阶段。以往研究发现,不同类型反馈(积极/消极反馈)和不同性质的强化物(物质/社会性强化)对儿童学习效果存在交互作用,并且对不同性别儿童的影响有所不同。本研究通过两个实验,采用联结学习范式以考察不同类型反馈对8~10岁儿童学习效果的影响。首先考察了积极反馈和消极反馈对儿童学习效果的影响,进而在积极、消极反馈的基础上加入了物质、社会性强化物,探究物质、社会性强化条件下积极、消极反馈对儿童学习效果影响的性别差异。结果表明,对于8~10岁儿童来说,消极反馈比积极反馈对儿童学习效果的影响更大。并且,物质、社会性强化物对儿童反馈学习效果的影响存在性别差异,对于男孩来说,在物质性强化条件下,消极反馈更能促进其学习;而对于女孩来说,在社会性强化物下,消极反馈更能促进她们的学习。该研究为如何运用反馈促进儿童学习提供了实证依据。

关键词: 积极反馈, 消极反馈, 物质性强化, 社会性强化, 性别差异

Abstract: The ability to learn from external feedback is particularly important during childhood. Previous studies have found that different types of feedback and reinforcement conditionings (the positive and negative feedback and the monetary and social reinforcement conditioning) had different influences on children's learning performances. In addition, there may be gender differences in the influences of the feedback type on learning performances. In this study, we used two experiments to examined the effects of the positive and negative feedback and the monetary and social feedback on children's learning performances. Experiment 1 examined effects of the positive and negative feedback on children's learning performances in 62 children (range 8-10 years). And experiment 2 examined the gender differences of learning performances in the monetary and social reinforcement conditionings in 133 children (range 8-10 years). These results indicated that, compared with positive feedback, negative feedback had a stronger influence on children's learning performances. In addition, the monetary reinforcement conditioning promoted boys' learning performances better, while the social reinforcement conditioning had a stronger influence on girls' learning performance.

Key words: positive feedback, negative feedback, monetary reinforcement conditioning, social reinforcement conditioning, gender differences

中图分类号: 

  • G442
Ashby, F. G., & O'Brien, J. B. (2007). The effects of positive versus negative feedback on information-integration category learning. Perception & Psychophysics, 69, 865-878.
Barth, H., Slusser, E., Kanjlia, S., Garcia, J., Taggart, J., & Chase, E. (2016). How feedback improves children's numerical estimation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23(4), 1198.
Bress, J. N., Smith, E., Foti, D., Klein, D. N., & Hajcak, G. (2012). Neural response to reward and depressive symptoms in late childhood to early adolescence. Biological psychology. 89(1), 156-162.
Chakrabarti, B., Kent, L., Suckling, J., Bullmore, E., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2006). Variations in the human cannabinoid receptor (CNR1) gene modulate striatal responses to happy faces. European Journal of Neuroscience, 23(7), 1944-1948.
Crowley, M. J., Wu, J., Bailey, C. A., & Mayes, L. C. (2009). Bringing in the negative reinforcements:the avoidance feedback-related negativity. Neuroreport, 20(17), 1513.
Crowley, M. J., Wu, J., Hommer, R. E., South, M., Molfese, P. J., Fearon R. M. P., & Mayes, L. C. (2013). A Developmental Study of the Feedback-Related Negativity From 10-17 Years:Age and Sex Effects for Reward Versus Non-Reward. Developmental Neuropsychology, 38(8), 595-612.
Delmonte, S., Balsters, J. H., Mcgrath, J., Fitzgerald, J., Brennan, S., & Fagan, A. J. (2012). Social and monetary reward processing in autism spectrum disorders. Molecular Autism, 3(1), 7.
Demurie, E., Roeyers, H., Baeyens, D., & Sonuga-Barke, E. (2012). The effects of monetary and social rewards on task performance in children and adolescents:liking is not enough. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 21(4), 301-310.
Ding, Y., Wang, E., Zou, Y., Song, Y., Xiao, X., & Huang, W. (2017). Gender differences in reward and punishment for monetary and social feedback in children:an ERP study. Plos One, 12(3), e0174100.
Domes, G., Schulze, L., Grossmann, A., Hauenstein, K., Wirtz, P. H., & Heinrichs, M.(2010). The neural correlates of sex differences in emotional reactivity and emotion regulation. Human Brain Mapping, 31(5), 758-769.
Duijvenvoorde, A. C. K. V., Jansen, B. R. J., Griffioen, E. S., Molen, M. W. V. D., & Huizenga, H. M. (2013). Decomposing developmental differences in probabilistic feedback learning:a combined performance and heart-rate analysis. Biological Psychology, 93(1), 175-183.
Eppinger, B., Mock, B., & Kray, J. (2009). Developmental differences in learning and error processing:evidence from ERPs. Psychophysiology, 46, 1043-1053.
Fehr, E., & Camerer, C. F. (2007). Social neuroeconomics:the neural circuitry of social preferences. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(10), 419.
Ferdinand, N. K., & Kray, J. (2014). Developmental changes in performance monitoring:How electrophysiological data can enhance our understanding of error and feedback processing in childhood and adolescence. Behavioural Brain Research, 263, 122-132.
Herbert, M., Eppinger, B., & Kray, J. (2011). To choose or to avoid:age differences in learning from positive and negative feedback. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(1), 41-52.
Higgins, E. T., Bond, R. N., Klein, R., & Strauman, T. (1986). Self-discrepancies and emotional vulnerability:how magnitude, accessibility, and type of discrepancy influence affect. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 51(1), 5.
Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. AmericanPsychologist, 52(12), 1280-1300.
Higgins, E. T. (2000). Making a good decision:Value from fit. American Psychologist, 55(11), 1217-1230.
Hoeft, F., Watson, C. L., Kesler, S. R., Bettinger, K. E., & Reiss, A. L. (2008). Gender differences in the mesocorticolimbic system during computer game-play. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 42(4), 253-258.
Hofmann, S. G., Suvak, M., & Litz, B. T. (2006). Sex differences in face recognition and influence of facial affect. Personality & Individual Differences, 40(8), 1683-1690.
Kamarajan, C., Rangaswamy, M., Chorlian, D. B., Manz, N., Tang, Y., Pandey, A. K., et al. (2008). Theta oscillations during the processing of monetary loss and gain:A perspective on gender and impulsivity. Brain Research, 1235, 45-62.
Kohls, G., Peltzer, J., Herpertz-Dahlmann, B., & Konrad, K. (2009). Differential effects of social and non-social reward on response inhibition in children and adolescents. Developmental Science, 12, 614-25.
Kujawa, A., Smith, E., Luhmann, C., & Hajcak. G. (2013). The feedback negativity reflects favorable compared to nonfavorable outcomes based on global, not local, alternatives. Psychophysiology. 50(2), 134-138.
Lang, A., & Betsch, T. (2018). Children's neglect of probabilities in decision making with and without feedback. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 191.
Lea, S. E., & Webley, P. (2006). Money as tool, money as drug:the biological psychology of a strong incentive. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 29(2), 161.
Li, F., Xie, L., Yang, X., & Cao, B. (2017). The effect of feedback and operational experience on children's rule learning. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 534.
Luking, K. R., Luby, J. L., & Barch, D. M. (2014). Kids, candy, brain and behavior:Age differences in responses to candy gains and losses. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 82-92.
Mcbridechang, C., Liu, P. D., Wong, T., Wong, A., & Shu, H. (2012). Specific reading difficulties in chinese, english, or both:longitudinal markers of phonological awareness, morphological awareness, and ran in hong kong chinese children. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 45(6), 503.
Pietschmann, M., Simon, K., Endrass, T., & Kathmann, N. (2008). Changes of performance monitoring with learning in older and younger adults. Psychophysiology, 45(4), 559-568.
Schmidt, L., & Segalowitz, S. (2008). Developmental psychophysiology. Cambridge University Press.
Schultz, W. (2004). Neural coding of basic reward terms of animal learning theory, game theory, microeconomics and behavioural ecology. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 14(2), 139.
Schwartz, F., & White, K. (2000). Making sense of it all:Giving and getting online course feedback. Boston:Allyn and Bacon.
Sorce, J. F., Emde, R. N., Campos, J. J., & Klinnert, M. D. (1985). Maternal emotional signaling:Its effect on the visual cliff behavior of 1-year-olds. Health and Wellbeing.
Stavropoulos, K. K., & Carver, L. J. (2014). Reward anticipation and processing of social versus nonsocial stimuli in children with and without autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 55(12):1398-1408.
Vohs, K. D., Mead, N. L., & Goode, M. R. (2006). The psychological consequences of money. Science, 314(5802), 1154.
Vrticčka, P., Sander, D., Anderson, B., Badoud, D., Eliez, S., & Debbané, M. (2014). Social feedback processing from early to late adolescence:influence of sex, age, and attachment style. Brain and Behavior, 4(5), 703.
Wang, D., Liu, T., & Shi, J. (2017). Development of monetary and social reward processes. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 1-10.
邸妙词, 刘儒德, 高钦, 王佳, 魏军, 唐铭. (2015). 小学生调节聚焦对生活满意度的影响:压力应对方式的中介作用.心理发展与教育, 31(3), 327-333.
龚栩, 黄宇霞, 王妍, 罗跃嘉. (2011). 中国面孔表情图片系统的修订. 中国心理卫生杂志, 25(1), 40-46.
龙君伟. (2003). 反馈干预对绩效的影响研究. 心理科学, 26(4), 658-660.
向玲, 王宝玺, 张庆林. (2012). 时间估计任务中的反馈加工和行为调节:来自ERP的证据. 心理学报, 44(9), 1149-1159.
[1] 郭嘉程, 董柔纯, 许放, 徐旋, 牛更枫, 周宗奎. 社会临场感与大学生网络过激行为的关系:双自我意识的并行中介及性别的调节作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(2): 176-186.
[2] 王玉龙, 赵婧斐, 蔺秀云. 家庭风险因素对青少年自伤的累积效应及其性别差异[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(2): 240-247.
[3] 邹盛奇, 伍新春. 父母冲突与青少年同伴依恋的关系:亲子依恋的中介作用及性别差异[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(6): 798-807.
[4] 赵纤, 王志航, 王东方, 袁言云, 尹霞云, 黎志华. 贫困家庭儿童在青少年早期的亲社会行为发展轨迹:性别及父母教养方式异质性的影响[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(3): 323-332.
[5] 李蓓蕾, 高婷, 张莉莉, 周楠, 邓林园. 学生感知的教师欺凌态度与学生欺凌行为的关系——学生欺凌态度的中介作用及其性别的调节作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2022, 38(3): 348-357.
[6] 白荣, 闫嵘, 王千, 李叶, 邢淑芬. 学前儿童执行功能与问题行为的关系:情境和性别的特异性[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2022, 38(1): 35-44.
[7] 姜永志, 白晓丽, 张璐, 赵思琦. 青少年线上积极自我呈现与幸福感的关系:线上积极反馈与自尊的作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2022, 38(1): 45-53.
[8] 任萍, 魏一, 孟晓哲, 秦幸娜, 王璇. 受欺负对抑郁情绪的影响:悲伤、愤怒反刍的中介作用及性别差异[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2021, 37(6): 873-881.
[9] 施国春, 赵东妍, 范会勇. 2004至2016年中国大学生身体自尊的变迁:一项横断历史研究[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2021, 37(5): 648-659.
[10] 窦芬, 李巧灵, 王书豪. 自我分化与大学生经验回避:羞怯的中介作用及其性别差异[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2021, 37(4): 517-524.
[11] 林悦, 刘勤学, 余思, 周宗奎. 父母忽视与青少年网络游戏成瘾的关系:希望的中介作用和性别的调节作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2021, 37(1): 109-119.
[12] 王苏, 盖笑松. 3~4岁幼儿冷热自我调节的发展趋势及性别差异——一项纵向研究[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2020, 36(6): 641-648.
[13] 赵旭, 彭茂滢, 杨娟. 内隐品质/外貌自我概念对内隐自尊的预测—性别的调节作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2020, 36(3): 296-303.
[14] 衡书鹏, 赵换方, 牛更枫, 周宗奎. 朋友圈点赞与主观幸福感的关系:一个有中介的调节模型[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2020, 36(3): 341-349.
[15] 丁倩, 张永欣, 周宗奎. 相对剥夺感与大学生网络过激行为:自我损耗的中介作用及性别差异[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2020, 36(2): 200-207.
Viewed
Full text


Abstract

Cited

  Shared   
  Discussed   
No Suggested Reading articles found!