Psychological Development and Education ›› 2020, Vol. 36 ›› Issue (4): 440-448.doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2020.04.07
Previous Articles Next Articles
ZHANG Hengchao
CLC Number:
Alibali, M. W., & Nathan, M. J. (2012). Embodiment in mathematics teaching and learning: Evidence from students’ and teachers’ gestures. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 21, 207-215. Arnold, J. E., Kahn, J. M., & Pancani, G. C. (2012). Audience design affects acoustic reduction via production facilitation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19(3), 505-512. Barnett, M, D., & Johnson, D, M. (2016). The perfectionism social disconnection model: The mediating role of communication styles. Personality and Individual Differences, 94, 200-205. Beyer, H., & Liebe, U. (2015). Three experimental approaches to measure the social context dependence of prejudice communication and discriminatory behavior. Social Science Research, 49, 343-355. Bezuidenhout, A. (2013). Perspective taking in conversation: a defense of speaker non-egocentricity. Journal of Pragmatics 48, 4-16. Brennan, S. E., Chen, X., Dickinson, C. A., Neider, M. B., & Zelinsky, G. J. (2008). Coordinating cognition: The costs and benefits of shared gaze during collaborative search. Cognition, 106(3), 1465-1477. Brown-Schmidt, S. (2009). Partner-specific interpretation of maintained referential precedents during interactive dialog. Journal of Memory and Language, 61(2), 171-190. Brown-Schmidt, S., & Heller, D. (2014). What language processing can tell us about perspective taking: A reply to Bezuidenhout (2013). Journal of Pragmatics, 60, 279-284. Buz, E., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Jaeger, T. F. (2016). Dynamically adapted context-specific hyper-articulation: Feedback from interlocutors affects speakers’ subsequent pronunciations. Journal of Memory and Language, 89, 68-86. Clark, H. H., & Wilkes-Gibbs, D. (1986). Referring as a collaborative process. Cognition, 22(1), 1-39. Crossley, L., Woodworth, M., Black, P. J., & Hare, R. (2016). The dark side of negotiation: Examining the outcomes of face-to-face and computer-mediated negotiations among dark personalities. Personality and Individual Differences, 91, 47-51. De Ruiter, J. P., Bangerter, A., & Dings, P. (2012). The interplay between gesture and speech in the production of referring expressions: Investigating the tradeoff hypothesis. Topics in Cognitive Science, 4(2), 232-248. Duran, N. D., & Dale, R. (2014). Perspective-taking in dialogue as self-organization under social constraints. New Ideas in Psychology, 32, 131-146. Gahl, S., & Strand, J. F. (2016). Many neighborhoods: Phonological and perceptual neighborhood density in lexical production and perception. Journal of Memory and Language, 89, 162-178. Galati, A. (2009). Assessing common ground in conversation: The effect of linguistic and physical co-presence on early planning (doctorial dissertation). State University of New York at Stony Brook. Galati, A., & Avraamides, M. N. (2013). Collaborating in spatial tasks: how partners coordinate their spatial memories and descriptions. Cognitive Processing,14(2), 1-3. Goldin-Meadow, S. (2015). From action to abstraction: Gesture as a mechanism of change. Developmental Review, 38, 167-184. Goldin-Meadow, S., & Alibali, M. W. (2013). Gesture’s role in speaking, learning, and creating. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 257-283. Graziano, M., & Gullberg, M. (2013). Gesture production and speech fluency in competent speakers and language learners. In Tilburg Gesture Research Meeting (TiGeR) 2013. Tilburg University. Hanna, J. E., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Trueswell, J. C. (2003). The effects of common ground and perspective on domains of referential interpretation. Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 43-61. Heller, D., Gorman, K. S., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2012). To name or to describe: shared knowledge affects referential form. Topics in Cognitive Science, 4(2), 290-305. Jacquette, D. (2014). Collective referential intentionality in the semantics of dialogue. Studies in Logic, Grammar and Rhetoric, 36 (49), 143-159. Keysar, B., Barr, D. J., Balin, J. A., & Brauner, J. S. (2000). Taking perspective in conversation: The role of mutual knowledge in comprehension. Psychological Science, 11(1), 32-38. Koppensteiner, M., Stephan, P., & Jäschke, J. P. M. (2016). Moving speeches: Dominance, trustworthiness and competence in body motion. Personality and Individual Differences, 94, 101-106. Krauss, R. M., & Weinheimer, S. (1964). Changes in reference phrases as a function of frequency of usage in social interaction: A preliminary study. Psychonomic Science, 1, 113-114. Kronmüller, E., & Barr, D. J. (2015). Referential precedents in spoken language comprehension: a review and meta-analysis. Journal of Memory and Language, 83, 1-19. Lewis, D. (1979). Scorekeeping in a language game. Journal of Philosophical Logic, 8(1), 339-359. Malt, B. C., & Sloman, S. A. (2004). Conversation and convention: Enduring influences on name choice for common objects. Memory & Cognition, 32(8), 1346-1354. Markman, A. B., & Makin, V. S. (1998). Referential communication and category acquisition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 127(4), 331-354. Nappa, R., & Arnold, J. E. (2014). The road to understanding is paved with the speaker’s intentions: Cues to the speaker’s attention and intentions affect pronoun comprehension. Cognitive Psychology, 70, 58-81. Novack, M., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2015). Learning from gesture: How our hands change our minds. Educational Psychology Review, 27(3), 405-412. O’Carroll, S., Nicoladis, E., & Smithson, L. (2015). The effect of extroversion on communication: Evidence from an interlocutor visibility manipulation. Speech Communication, 69, 1-8. Overall, N. C., & McNulty, J. K. (2017). What type of communication during conflict is beneficial for intimate relationships? Current Opinion in Psychology, 13, 1-5. Roberts, G., Langstein, B., & Galantucci, B. (2016). (In)sensitivity to incoherence in human communication. Language & Communication, 47, 15-22. Rogers, S. L., Fay, N., & Maybery, M. (2013). Audience design through social interaction during group discussion. PloS one, 8(2), e57211. Sidera, F., Serrat, E., Serrano, J., Rostan, C., Caño, A., & Amadó, A. (2013). Let’s share perspectives! Mentalistic skills involved in cooperation. International Journal of Educational Psychology, 2(3), 325-352. Vanlangendonck, F., Willems, R. M., Menenti, L., & Hagoort, P. (2013). The role of common ground in audience design: Beyond an all or nothing story. In the Workshop on the Production of Referring Expressions: Bridging the Gap between Computational and Empirical Approaches to Reference the (PRE-CogSci 2013). Vesper, C., Schmitz, L., Safra, L., Sebanz, N., & Knoblich, G. (2016). The role of shared visual information for joint action coordination. Cognition, 153, 118-123. Yoon, S. O., Koh, S., & Brown-Schmidt. (2012). Influence of perspective and goals on reference production in conversation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 699-707. Yu, C., Schermerhorn, P., & Scheutz, M. (2012). Adaptive eye gaze patterns in interactions with human and artificial agents. Transactions on Interactive Intelligent Systems, 1(2), 13-43. Zwaan, R. A.(2014). Embodiment and language comprehension: reframing the discussion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(5), 229-234. 张恒超. (2013). 参照性交流中的“听者设计”. 心理发展与教育, 29(5), 552-560. 张恒超. (2017a). 共享因素对参照性交流双方学习的影响. 心理学报, 49(2), 197-205. 张恒超. (2017b). 参照性交流学习中语言参照惯例的形成特点. 西南大学学报(自然科学版), 39(10), 133-138. 张恒超. (2018a). 交流语言认知特征. 心理科学进展, 26(2), 270-282. 张恒超. (2018b). 交流手势的认知特征. 心理科学进展, 26(5), 796-809. 张恒超, 阴国恩. (2012). 关系复杂性对关系类别间接性学习的影响. 心理发展与教育, 28(2), 193-200. 张恒超, 阴国恩. (2014). 参照性交流中的非策略性认知过程. 心理研究, 7(5), 7-14. |
|