Psychological Development and Education ›› 2019, Vol. 35 ›› Issue (3): 320-328.doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2019.03.09
Previous Articles Next Articles
ZHANG Hengchao
CLC Number:
Abney, D. H., Paxton, A., Dale, R., & Kello, C. T. (2014). Complexity matching in dyadic conversation. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 143(6), 2304-2315. Alibali, M. W., Nathan, M. J., Wolfgram, M. S., Church, R. B., Jacobs, S. A., Johnson Martinez, C., & Knuth, E. J. (2014). How teachers link ideas in mathematics instruction using speech and gesture:a corpus analysis. Cognition and Instruction, 32, 65-100. Arnold, J. E., Kahn, J. M., & Pancani, G. C. (2012). Audience design affects acoustic reduction via production facilitation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19(3), 505-512. Beaudoin-Ryan, L., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2014). Teaching moral reasoning through gesture. Developmental Science, 17(6), 984-990. Bezuidenhout, A. (2013). Perspective taking in conversation:a defense of speaker non-egocentricity. Journal of Pragmatics 48, 4-16. Brennan, S. E. (2005). How conversation is shaped by visual and spoken evidence. In J. Trueswell & M. Tanenhaus (Eds.), Approaches to Studying World-Situated Language Use:Bridging the Language-as-Product and Language-Action Traditions (95-129). Cambridge, MA:MIT Press. Brennan, S. E., Chen, X., Dickinson, C. A., Neider, M. B., & Zelinsky, G. J. (2008). Coordinating cognition:The costs and benefits of shared gaze during collaborative search. Cognition, 106(3), 1465-1477. Brennan, S. E., & Hanna, J. E. (2009). Partner-specific adaptation in dialog. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1(2), 274-291. Brown-Schmidt, S., & Heller, D. (2014). What language processing can tell us about perspective taking:A reply to Bezuidenhout (2013). Journal of Pragmatics, 60, 279-284. Cook, S. W., Duffy, R. G., & Fenn, K. M. (2013). Consolidation and transfer of learning after observing hand gesture. Child Development, 84(6), 1863-1871. Cooperrider, K., Wakefield, E., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2015). More than meets the eye:Gesture changes thought, even without visual feedback. In R. Dale, C. Jennings, P. P. Maglio, T. Matlock, D. Noelle, A. Warlaumont, & J. Yoshimi (Eds.), Proceedings of the 37th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (441-446). Austin, TX:Cognitive Science Society. Fay, N., Walker, B., Swoboda, N., & Garrod, S. (2018). How to create shared symbols. Language, 13(1), 1-29. Ferguson, H. J., Apperly, I. A., Ahmad, J., & Bindermann, M. (2015). Task constraints distinguish perspective inferences from perspective use during discourse interpretation. Cognition, 139, 50-70. Fusaroli, R., Raczaszek-Leonardi, J., & Tylén, K. (2014). Dialog as interpersonal synergy. New Ideas in Psychology, 32, 147-157. Galati, A. (2009). Assessing common ground in conversation:The effect of linguistic and physical co-presence on early planning (doctorial dissertation). Stony Brook University, New York. Goldin-Meadow, S. (2015). From action to abstraction:Gesture as a mechanism of change. Developmental Review, 38, 167-184. Goldin-Meadow, S., & Alibali, M. W. (2013). Gesture's role in speaking, learning, and creating. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 257-283. Graziano, M., & Gullberg, M. (2013). Gesture production and speech fluency in competent speakers and language learners. In Tilburg Gesture Research Meeting (TiGeR) 2013. Tilburg University. Hostetter, A. B. (2011). When do gestures communicate-A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 137(2), 297-315. Hu, F. T., Ginns, P., & Bobis, J. (2015). Getting the point:tracing worked examples enhances learning. Learning and Instruction, 35, 85-93. James, K. H., & Swain, S. N. (2011). Only self-generated actions create sensori-motor systems in the developing brain. Developmental Psychology, 14, 1-6. Kita S., & Özyürek, A. (2003).What does cross-linguistic variation in semantic coordination of speech and gesture reveal? Evidence for an interface representation of spatial thinking and speaking. Journal of Memory and Language. 48,16-32. Koppensteiner, M., Stephan, P., & Jäschke, J. P. M. (2016). Moving speeches:Dominance, trustworthiness and competence in body motion. Personality and Individual Differences, 94, 101-106. Krauss, R. M. (1998). Why do we gesture when we speak? Current Directions in Psychological Science. 7(2), 54-60. Krauss, R. M., & Weinheimer, S. (1964). Changes in reference phrases as a function of frequency of usage in social interaction:A preliminary study. Psychonomic Science, 1, 113-114. Markman, A. B., & Makin, V. S. (1998). Referential communication and category acquisition. Journal of Experimental Psychology:General, 127(4), 331-354. Mol, L., & Kita, S. (2012). Gesture structure affects syntactic structure in speech. In Naomi Miyake, David Peebles, & Richard P. Cooper (Eds.), Proceedings of the 34th Annual Conference of The Cognitive Science Society (761-766). Austin, TX:Cognitive Science Society. Molenberghs, P., Johnson, H., Henry, J. D., & Mattingley, J. B. (2016). Understanding the minds of others:A neuroimaging meta-analysis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 65, 276-291. Mumford, K. H., & Kita, S. (2014). Children use gesture to interpret novel verb meanings. Child Development, 85(3), 1181-1189. Novack, M., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2015). Learning from gesture:How our hands change our minds. Educational Psychology Review, 27(3), 405-412. O'Carroll, S., Nicoladis, E., & Smithson, L. (2015). The effect of extroversion on communication:Evidence from an interlocutor visibility manipulation. Speech Communication, 69, 1-8. Pine, K. J, Bird, H & Kirk, E. (2007). The effects of prohibiting gestures on children's lexical retrieval ability. Developmental Science, 10(6), 747-754. Rogers, S. L., Fay, N., & Maybery, M. (2013). Audience design through social interaction during group discussion. PloS one, 8(2), e57211. Theisen, C. A., Oberlander, J., & Kirby, S. (2010). Systematicity and arbitrariness in novel communication systems. Interaction Studies, 11(1), 14-32. Vanlangendonck, F., Willems, R. M., Menenti, L., & Hagoort, P. (2013). The role of common ground in audience design:Beyond an all or nothing story. In the Workshop on the Production of Referring Expressions:Bridging the Gap between Computational and Empirical Approaches to Reference the (PRE-CogSci 2013). Vesper, C., Schmitz, L., Safra, L., Sebanz, N., & Knoblich, G. (2016). The role of shared visual information for joint action coordination. Cognition, 153, 118-123. Vigliocco, G., Perniss, P., & Vinson, D. (2014). Language as a multimodal phenomenon:Implications for language learning, processing, and evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 369, 20130292. Wakefield, E.M., Hall, C., James, K.H., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2017). Representational gesture as a tool for promoting word learning in young children. In Proceedings of the 41st Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, Boston, MA. Zhou, H., Majka, E. A., & Epley, N. (2017). Inferring perspective versus getting perspective:Underestimating the value of being in another person's shoes. Psychological Science, 28(4), 482-493. 沈德立, 白学军. (2006). 高效率学习的心理机制研究. 心理科学, 29(1), 2-6. 张恒超. (2013). 参照性交流中的"听者设计". 心理发展与教育, 29(5), 552-560. 张恒超. (2017). 共享因素对参照性交流双方学习的影响. 心理学报, 49(2), 197-205. 张恒超. (2018a). 交流语言认知特征. 心理科学进展, 26(2), 270-282. 张恒超. (2018b). 交流手势的认知特征. 心理科学进展, 26(5), 796-809. 张恒超, 阴国恩. (2012a). 关系复杂性对关系类别间接性学习的影响. 心理发展与教育, 28(2), 193-200. 张恒超, 阴国恩. (2012b). 关系复杂性对关系类别间接性学习中选择性注意的影响. 心理科学, 35(4), 823-828. |
|