心理发展与教育 ›› 2021, Vol. 37 ›› Issue (4): 481-488.doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2021.04.04
王菡1, 俞稼钰2, 梁丹丹2
WANG Han1, YU Jiayu2, LIANG Dandan2
摘要: 采用句子理解任务分别探究了5~7岁的汉语高功能自闭症儿童在单独提及顺序线索,提及顺序线索和重读线索共现两种条件下代词加工的表现。研究结果表明,汉语高功能自闭症儿童不像智商和工作记忆广度匹配的同龄典型发展儿童一样能够单独利用提及顺序线索加工代词,即倾向于选择首先提及的实体作为代词的指称对象,而表现为随机选择代词所指。当提及顺序线索和重读线索共现时,典型发展儿童在两条线索一致和冲突时都倾向于依靠重读线索加工代词,而汉语高功能自闭症儿童对两条线索都不敏感,表现为随机选择代词所指。这表明5~7岁的汉语高功能儿童不能够利用这两条线索加工代词,在利用语篇突显性线索加工代词方面存在困难。
中图分类号:
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA:American Psychiatric Publishing. Ariel, M. (1990). Accessing noun-phrase antecedents(pp.56-64). London:Routledge. Cascadilla Press. Arnold, J. E. (1998). Reference form and discourse patterns (unpublished doctoral dissertation). Stanford University. Arnold, J. E. (2001). The effect of thematic roles on pronoun use and frequency of reference continuation. Discourse Processes, 31(2), 137-162. Arnold, J. E., Bennetto, L., & Diehl, E. (2009). Reference production in young speakers with and without autism:Effects of discourse status and processing constraints. Cognition, 110(2), 131-46. Balogh, J. E. (2003). Pronouns, Prosody, and the Discourse Anaphora Weighting Approach (unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of California. Brehme, D. (2014). Perspectives on Personal Pronoun Reversal in Children with ASD:A Critical Review. Journal of European Psychology Students, 5(1), 31-37. Colle, L., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., & Van der Lely, H. (2008). Narrative Discourse in Adults with High-Functioning Autism or Asperger Syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(1), 28-40. Edelson, L. R. (2011). Cues to Pronominal Reference Resolution in Children with and Without Autism Spectrum Disorders (unpublished doctoral dissertation). Boston University. Gernsbacher, M. A., & Hargreaves, D. J. (1988). Accessing sentence participants:The advantage of first mention. Journal of Memory and Language, 27(6), 699-717. Grice, H.P. (1975). Logic and Conversation. In R. Cole & J. Morgan (Eds.), Syntax and Semantics:Speech Acts(pp.41-58). New York:Academic Press. Gundel, J. K., Hedberg, N., & Zacharski, R. (1993). Cognitive status and the form of referring expressions in discourse. Language, 69(2), 274-307. Hartshorne, J. K., Nappa, R., & Snedeker, J. (2015). Development of the first-mention bias. Journal of Child Language, 42(2), 423-446. Itzhak, I. & Baum, S. R. (2015). Misleading Bias-Driven Expectations in Referential Processing and the Facilitative Role of Contrastive Accent. Journal of Psycholinguist Research. 44(5), 623-650. Järvinen-Pasley, A., Peppé, S., King-Smith, G., & Heaton, P. (2008). The Relationship between Form and Function Level Receptive Prosodic Abilities in Autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(7), 1328-1340. Kertoy, M. K. (1991). Listening Comprehension for Sentences:the Accessibility of Referents for Pronouns as a Function of Age, Topic Continuity, and Pronoun Emphasis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 52(3), 344-353. Lombardo, M. V., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2010). Unravelling the paradox of the autistic self. Cognitive Science, 1(3), 393-403. Maratsos, M. P. (1973). The effects of stress on the understanding of pronominal coreference in children. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2(1), 1-8. Nieuwland, M. S., & van Berkum, J. J. A. (2006). Individual differences and contextual bias in pronoun resolution:Evidence from ERPs. Brain Research, 1118(1), 155-167. Novogrodsky, R. (2013). Subject pronoun used by children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD). Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 27(2), 85-93. Peppé, S., McCann, J., Gibbon, F., O'Hare, A., & Rutherford, M. (2007). Receptive and Expressive Prosodic Ability in Children with High-Functioning Autism. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 50(4), 1015. Snedeker, J., & Trueswell, John C. (2004). The developing constraints on parsing decisions:The role of lexical-biases and referential scenes in child and adult sentence processing. Cognitive Psychology, 49(3), 238-299. Solan, L. (1980). Contrastive Stress and Children's Interpretation of Pronouns. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 25(3), 688-698. Song, H. (2004). The Influence of Discourse Representation of Young Children's Pronoun Interpretation (unpublished doctoral dissertation). Seoul National University. Song, H., & Fisher, C. (2005). Who's ‘she’? Discourse prominence influences preschoolers' comprehension of pronouns. Journal of Memory and Language, 52(1), 29-57. Song, H., & Fisher, C. (2007). Discourse prominence effects on 2.5-year-old children's interpretation of pronouns. Lingua, 777(11), 1959-1987. Stevenson, R. J., Crawley, R. A., & Kleinman, D. (1994). Thematic roles, focus and the representation of events. Language and Cognitive Processes, 9(4), 473-592. Swaab, T. Y., Camblin, C. C, & Gordon, P. C. (2004). Electrophysiological evidence for reversed lexical repetition effects in language processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(5), 715-726. Terzi, A., Marinis, T., & Francis, K. (2016). The interface of syntax with pragmatics and prosody in children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism Development Disorder, 46(8), 2692-2706. Van Rij, J., Hollebrandse, B., & Hendriks, P. (2016). Children's eye gaze reveals their use of discourse context in object pronoun resolution(pp.267-293). Berlin/Boston:De Gruyter. 陈伟英, 皮姆·马克, 泰德·桑德斯.(2013).句法角色与话题性对实体突显性的影响.浙江大学学报(人文社会科学版), 43(1), 122-134. 李伟亚. (2009). 自闭症谱系障碍学生汉语句子理解过程的实验研究(博士学位论文). 华东师范大学, 上海. 缪小春. (1996). 影响代词加工的语义和语法因素研究. 心理学报, 28(4), 352-358. 邵敬敏. (主编)(2007). 现代汉语通论(pp.180-184). 第二版. 上海:上海教育出版社. 郑波, 王蓓, 杨玉芳. (2002). 韵律对指代歧义的解歧作用及其机制. 心理学报, 34(6), 567-572. |
[1] | 林崇德. 需要是教育与发展的动力源[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(1): 1-7. |
[2] | 冷欣怡, 苏萌萌, 李文玲, 杨秀杰, 邢爱玲, 张湘琳, 舒华. 家庭环境与农村儿童早期语言发展的关系[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(1): 8-18. |
[3] | 陈书玲, 贾会宾, 靳璨, 张欣, 王恩国. 发展性协调障碍儿童视觉通道前注意加工的特点:来自ERP的证据[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(1): 19-28. |
[4] | 谢和平, 王燕青, 王福兴, 周宗奎, 邓素娥, 段朝辉. 记忆的生成绘图效应及其边界条件:一项元分析[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(1): 29-43. |
[5] | 田惠东, 张玉红, 孙昊翔, 李瑛, 哈尼夏. 师生关系对智力障碍学生同伴交往能力的影响:有调节的中介模型[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(1): 44-54. |
[6] | 陈启山, 何静怡. 女大学生室友的愤怒特质匹配与人际关系:有调节的多项式回归模型[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(1): 55-63. |
[7] | 王鑫强, 李金文, 卢红燕, 赖正伟, 李佳源. 择偶优劣势对师范大学生教师职业认同的影响:择偶价值感的中介及自尊的调节作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(1): 64-73. |
[8] | 赵京伟, 陈晓旭, 任立文, 耿喆, 徐夫真. 父母心理控制与小学儿童焦虑:一个有调节的中介模型[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(1): 93-102. |
[9] | 秦瑶, 彭运石. 父母教养方式对初中生社交焦虑的影响:同伴接纳和反刍思维的链式中介作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(1): 103-113. |
[10] | 魏华, 丁慧敏, 陈武, 郝兴风, 熊婕. 父母低头行为与青少年网络欺负的关系:压力的中介作用与年龄的调节作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(1): 114-121. |
[11] | 樊香麟, 崔英锦. 客体化身体意识与女大学生限制性饮食行为的关系:外貌负面评价恐惧和社会文化压力的中介作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(1): 122-131. |
[12] | 张慧如, 张伟达, 傅王倩, 邓敏, 彭苏浩, 李玉. 孤独感对创造性倾向的影响:无聊倾向和焦虑情绪的中介作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(1): 132-141. |
[13] | 王伊萌, 王振东, 汪凤炎. “当局者迷”破解之道:智慧的正念干预研究——个人成长主动性与反思的作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(1): 142-152. |
[14] | 金星, 刘景弘, 马跃, 于战宇. 聋童和正常儿童身体表情注意瞬脱的比较研究[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(3): 305-312. |
[15] | 钟伟芳, 郭永兴. 语言范畴影响面部表情知觉的电生理学证据[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(3): 313-322. |
|