心理发展与教育 ›› 2017, Vol. 33 ›› Issue (5): 524-534.doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2017.05.02
童钰, 王福兴
TONG Yu, WANG Fuxing
摘要: 大量运用视觉搜索范式的研究发现,在进化过程中对人类生存具有威胁的蛇会被人们更快地觉察。但已有研究采用动物(蛇)植物(花)配对搜索的方式,而很少直接采用近似的动物配对搜索。本研究选取成人大学生被试,通过视觉搜索范式和眼动追踪,对比了蛇和同为爬行类动物、却不具有进化上威胁性的蜥蜴的视觉搜索过程,以探究蛇的威胁性和动物特征相似性在威胁性刺激注意偏向中的作用。实验1采用蛇和蜥蜴互为目标物和干扰物,发现相对于蛇,被试对蜥蜴的行为反应时更短;首次注视到达时间也更短。实验2-4分别采用黑白和线画刺激、恒定干扰物(花和青蛙)、使用自然背景材料对比蛇和蜥蜴的觉察,发现了一致的结果,即被试对蜥蜴的视觉搜索时间和行为反应时间都要快于蛇。在视觉搜索任务中当两种视觉刺激材料的属性接近时,蛇在进化上的威胁性不一定导致更快的搜索时间,而刺激物的视觉特征对目标物的觉察会产生重要的影响。
中图分类号:
B844
Bjärtå, A., Flykt, A., & Sundin, Ö. (2013). The effect of using different distractor sets in visual search with spiders and snakes on spider-sensitive and nonfearful participants. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 72(4), 171-179.Blanchette, I. (2006). Snakes, spiders, guns, and syringes:How specific are evolutionary constraints on the detection of threatening stimuli? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59(8), 1484-1504.Brosch, T., & Sharma, D. (2005). The role of fear-relevant stimuli in visual search:A comparison of phylogenetic and ontogenetic stimuli. Emotion, 5(3), 360-364.Calvillo, D. P., & Hawkins, W. C. (2016). Animate objects are detected more frequently than inanimate objects in inattentional blindness tasks independently of threat. The Journal of General Psychology, 143(2), 101-115.DeLoache, J. S., & LoBue, V. (2009). The narrow fellow in the grass:Human infants associate snakes and fear. Developmental Science, 12(1), 201-207.Flykt, A. (2006). Preparedness for action:Responding to the snake in the grass. The American Journal of Psychology, 119(1), 29-43.Flykt, A., & Caldara, R. (2006). Tracking fear in snake and spider fearful participants during visual search:A multi-response domain study. Cognition and Emotion, 20(8), 1075-1091.Forbes, S. J., Purkis, H. M., & Lipp, O. V. (2011). Better safe than sorry:Simplistic fear-relevant stimuli capture attention. Cognition and Emotion, 25(5), 794-804.Fox, E., Griggs, L., & Mouchlianitis, E. (2007). The detection of fear-relevant stimuli:Are guns noticed as quickly as snakes? Emotion, 7(4), 691-696.Hayakawa, S., Kawai, N., & Masataka, N. (2011). The influence of color on snake detection in visual search in human children. Scientific Reports, 80(1), 1-4.Isbell, L. A. (2006). Snakes as agents of evolutionary change in primate brains. Journal of Human Evolution, 51(1), 1-35.Lipp, O. V., Derakshan, N., Waters, A. M., & Logies, S. (2004). Snakes and cats in the flower bed:Fast detection is not specific to pictures of fear-relevant animals. Emotion, 4(3), 233-250.Lipp, O. V., & Waters, A. M. (2007). When danger lurks in the background:Attentional capture by animal fear-relevant distractors is specific and selectively enhanced by animal fear. Emotion, 7(1), 192-200.LoBue, V. (2013). What are we so afraid of? How early attention shapes our most common fears. Child Development Perspectives, 7(1), 38-42.LoBue, V. (2014). Deconstructing the snake:The relative roles of perception, cognition, and emotion on threat detection. Emotion, 14(4), 701-711.LoBue, V., & DeLoache, J. S. (2008). Detecting the Snake in the Grass Pychological Science, 19(3), 284-289.LoBue, V., & DeLoache, J. S. (2010). Superior detection of threat-relevant stimuli in infancy. Developmental Science, 13(1), 221-228.LoBue, V., & DeLoache, J. S. (2011). What's so special about slithering serpents? Children and adults rapidly detect snakes based on their simple features. Visual Cognition, 19(1), 129-143.LoBue, V., & Matthews, K. (2014). The snake in the grass revisited:An experimental comparison of threat detection paradigms. Cognition and Emotion, 28(1), 22-35.LoBue, V., Matthews, K., Harvey, T., & Stark, S. L. (2014). What accounts for the rapid detection of threat? Evidence for an advantage in perceptual and behavioral responding from eye movements. Emotion, 14(4), 816-823.LoBue, V., & Rakison, D. H. (2013). What we fear most:A developmental advantage for threat-relevant stimuli. Developmental Review, 33(4), 285-303.LoBue, V., Rakison, D. H., & DeLoache, J. S. (2010). Threat perception across the life span:Evidence for multiple converging pathways. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19(6), 375-379.Mallan, K. M., Lipp, O. V., & Cochrane, B. (2013). Slithering snakes, angry men and out-group members:What and whom are we evolved to fear? Cognition and Emotion, 27(7), 1168-1180.Marks, K. R., Roberts, W., Stoops, W. W., Pike, E., Fillmore, M. T., & Rush, C. R. (2014). Fixation time is a sensitive measure of cocaine cue attentional bias. Addiction, 109(9), 1501-1508.Masataka, N., Hayakawa, S., & Kawai, N. (2010). Human young children as well as adults demonstrate ‘superior’ rapid snake detection when typical striking posture is displayed by the snake. PLoS One, 5(11), e15122.New, J., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2007). Category-specific attention for animals reflects ancestral priorities, not expertise. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104(42), 16598-16603.Öhman, A. (2009). Of snakes and faces:An evolutionary perspective on the psychology of fear. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 50(6), 543-552.Öhman, A., Flykt, A., & Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention:Detecting the snake in the grass. Journal of Experimental Psychology:General, 130(3), 466-478.Öhman, A., & Mineka, S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness:Toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychological Review, 108(3), 483-522.Öhman, A., & Mineka, S. (2003). The malicious serpent:Snakes as a prototypical stimulus for an evolved module of fear. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12(1), 5-9.Penkunas, M. J., & Coss, R. G. (2013a). A comparison of rural and urban Indian children's visual detection of threatening and nonthreatening animals. Developmental Science, 16(3), 463-475.Penkunas, M. J., & Coss, R. G. (2013b). Rapid detection of visually provocative animals by preschool children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114(4), 522-536.Rachman, S. (1977). The conditioning theory of fear acquisition:A critical examination. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 15, 375-387.Rakison, D. H., & Derringer, J. (2007). Do infants possess an evolved spider-detection mechanism? Cognition, 107(1), 381-393.Rayner, K. (1998). Eye movements in reading and information processing:20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin, 124(3), 372-422.Shen, J., & Reingold, E. M. (2001). Visual search asymmetry:The influence of stimulus familiarity and low-level features. Perception and Psychophysics, 63(3), 464-475.Soares, S. C., Esteves, F., Lundqvist, D., & Öhman, A. (2009). Some animal specific fears are more specific than others:Evidence from attention and emotion measures. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47(12), 1032-1042.Soares, S. C., Lindström, B., Esteves, F., & Öhman, A. (2014). The hidden snake in the grass:Superior detection of snakes in challenging attentional conditions. PLoS One, 9(12), e114724.Thrasher, C., & LoBue, V. (2016). Do infants find snakes aversive? Infants'physiological responses to "fear-relevant" stimuli. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 142, 382-390.Tipples, J., Young, A. W., Quinlan, P., Broks, P., & Ellis, A. W. (2002). Searching for threat. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,55A(3), 1007-1026.Treisman, A., & Gormican, S. (1998). Feature analysis in early vision:Evidence from search asymmetries. Psychological Review, 95(1), 15-48.Treisman, A., & Souther, J. (1985). Search asymmetry:A diagnostic for preattentive processing of separable features. Journal of Experimental Psychology:General, 114(3), 285-310.Waters, A. M., Lipp, O., & Spence, S. H. (2008). Visual search for animal fear-relevant stimuli in children. Australian Journal of Psychology, 60(2), 112-125.Yang, J., Wang, A., Yan, M., Zhu, Z., Chen, C., & Wang, Y. (2012). Distinct processing for pictures of animals and objects:Evidence from eye movements. Emotion, 12(3), 540-551.Yorzinski, J. L., Penkunas, M. J., Platt, M. L., & Coss, R. G. (2014). Dangerous animals capture and maintain attention in humans. Evolutionary Psychology, 12(3), 534-548.王福兴, 李文静, 颜志强, 段朝辉, 李卉. (2015). 幼儿对威胁性刺激蛇的注意觉察:来自眼动证据. 心理学报, 47(6), 774-786.王福兴, 童钰, 钱莹莹, 谢和平. (2016). 眼动追踪技术与婴幼儿研究:程序、方法与数据分析. 心理与行为研究, 14(4), 558-567. |
[1] | 王敬欣, 赵赛男, 徐倩倩. 字间空格与词频对青年人和老年人阅读的影响:眼动研究[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(6): 781-787. |
[2] | 高子惠, 焦雨, 王曦, 刘肖岑. 电子绘本文字的动静态呈现方式对幼儿阅读体验和学习效果的影响[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(6): 817-824. |
[3] | 于晓, 张涵, 陈英和, 戚玥, 刘爱芳, 刘丽丽. 类比推理的眼动研究:揭示个体类比推理策略发展的有效手段[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2021, 37(6): 897-903. |
[4] | 施芳婷, 郑晨烨, 颜秀琳, 陆露, 王静梅, 邸波, 卢英俊. 5~6岁幼儿对不同文化背景卡通面孔再认的眼动研究[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2021, 37(3): 323-334. |
[5] | 马安然, 王燕青, 王福兴, 周治金. 教学微视频的播放速度对学习效果的影响[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2021, 37(3): 391-399. |
[6] | 辛聪, 张曼曼, 郭盈秀, 郭云飞, 陈幼贞. 前瞻记忆意向后效应的加工机制:来自眼动的证据[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2020, 36(2): 138-145. |
[7] | 刘志方, 仝文, 张骏. 中文阅读中词汇加工的年老化:眼动证据[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2019, 35(6): 665-677. |
[8] | 王薇, 徐知宇, 李永鑫, 程奕芸. 情绪主题绘本阅读对自闭症谱系障碍儿童情绪理解障碍的干预效果[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2019, 35(5): 566-572. |
[9] | 张骏, 仝文, 刘志方. 不同词长中文句子阅读知觉广度的年老化:眼动证据[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2019, 35(3): 312-319. |
[10] | 陈朝阳, 刘志方, 苏永强, 程亚华. 高低阅读技能聋生词汇加工的语境预测性效应特点:眼动证据[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2018, 34(6): 692-699. |
[11] | 苏永强, 付福音, 刘志方, 陈朝阳. 阅读中词汇视觉编码年老化的原因:基于消失文本实验的证据[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2017, 33(4): 433-440. |
[12] | 李文静, 童钰, 王福兴, 康素杰, 刘华山, 杨超. 动画教学代理对多媒体学习的影响:学习者经验与偏好的调节作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2016, 32(4): 453-462. |
[13] | 钱莹莹, 王福兴, 段朝辉, 周宗奎. 动画速度和学习者经验对多媒体学习的影响[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2016, 32(2): 191-197. |
[14] | 张玉晶, 买合甫来提·坎吉, 阻木然提古丽·然木吐拉. 不同水平五年级维吾尔族学生母语阅读知觉广度[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2015, 31(6): 703-709. |
[15] | 白学军, 郭志英, 王永胜, 高晓雷, 闫国利. 老年人与青年人阅读空格文本的注视位置效应[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2015, 31(2): 171-179. |
|