心理发展与教育 ›› 2020, Vol. 36 ›› Issue (3): 257-264.doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2020.03.01
• 认知与社会性发展 • 下一篇
韩瑽瑽1, 陈英和2, 于晓2, 邓之君3, 刘静4, 侯江文5, 林燕燕6
HAN Congcong1, CHEN Yinghe2, YU Xiao2, DENG Zhijun3, LIU Jing4, HOU Jiangwen5, LIN Yanyan6
摘要: 研究采用数量关系匹配任务和相对大小关系匹配任务,通过高表面相似性、低表面相似性和交叉映射三种实验条件,探讨表面相似性对儿童和成人数量与相对大小关系理解的影响。研究一对37名4岁儿童进行三次每次间隔半年的追踪测查,结果表明4~5岁间儿童理解数量关系和相对大小关系的水平有显著提高;4岁儿童对数量关系的理解受到表面相似性的影响,4~5岁儿童对相对大小关系的理解受到表面相似性的影响。研究二探究44名成人数量和相对大小关系理解的特点,结果发现,成人完成两种关系匹配任务时也受到表面相似性的影响。
中图分类号:
Aïte, A., Berthoz, A., Vidal, J., Roëll, M., Zaoui, M., & Houdé, O., et al. (2016). Taking a third-person perspective requires inhibitory control:evidence from a developmental negative priming study. Child Development, 87(6), 1825-1840. Blanchette, I., & Dunbar, K. (2000). How analogies are generated:the roles of structural and superficial similarity. Memory & Cognition, 28(1), 108-124. Borst, G., Poirel, N., Pineau, A., Cassotti, M., & Houdé, O. (2013). Inhibitory control efficiency in a piaget-like class-inclusion task in school-age children and adults:a developmental negative priming study. Developmental Psychology, 49(7), 1366-1374. Carlson, S. M. (2005). Developmentally sensitive measures of executive function in preschool children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28(2), 595-616. Catrambone, R. (2002). The effects of surface and structural feature matches on the access of story analogs. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28(2), 318-334. Clearfield, M. W., & Mix, K. S. (2001). Amount versus number:infants' use of area and contour length to discriminate small sets. Journal of Cognition and Development, 2(3), 243-260. Daurignac, E., Houdé, O., & Jouvent, R. (2006). Negative priming in a numerical piaget-like task as evidenced by ERP. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(5), 730-736. Deng, W., & Sloutsky, V. M. (2016). Selective attention, diffused attention, and the development of categorization. Cognitive Psychology, 91, 24-62. Dumas, D. (2017). Relational reasoning in science, medicine, and engineering. Educational Psychology Review, 29(1), 73-95. Feigenson, L., Carey, S., & Spelke, E.S.(2002). Infants' discrimination of number vs. continuous extent. Cognitive Psychology, 44(1), 33-66. Gelman, R. (1998). Domain specificity in cognitive development:Universals and nouniversals. IN Sabourin, M., Craik, F. and Robert, M. (Eds) Advances in psychological science. Psychology Press Ltd., Vol 2, (25), 557-579. Gentner, D., & Medina, J. (1998). Similarity and the development of rules. Cognition, 65(2-3), 263-297. Gentner, D. (1988). Metaphor as structure mapping:the relational shift. Child Development, 59(1), 47-59. Ghetti, S., & Lee, J. (2011). Children's episodic memory. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews Cognitive Science, 2(4), 365-373. Grossnickle, E. M., Dumas, D., Alexander, P. A., & Baggetta, P. (2016). Individual differences in the process of relational reasoning. Learning & Instruction, 42, 141-159. Hanania, R., & Smith, L. B. (2010). Selective attention and attention switching:towards a unified developmental approach. Developmental Science, 13(4), 622-635. Haryu, E., Imai, M., & Okada, H. (2011). Object similarity bootstraps young children to action-based verb extension. Child Development, 82(2), 674-686. Houdé, O., Pineau, A., Leroux, G., Poirel, N., Perchey, G., Lanoe, C., et al. (2011). Functional MRI study of Piaget's conservation-of-number task in preschool and school-age children:a neo-Piagetian approach. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 110, 332-346. Landau, B., Smith, L., & Jones, S. (1998). Object shape, object function, and object name. Journal of Memory & Language, 38(1), 1-27. Lubin, A., Simon, G., Houdé, O., & Neys, W. D. (2015). Inhibition, conflict detection, and number conservation. International Journal on mathematics education, 47(5), 793-800. Markman, A. B., & Ross, B. H. (2003). Category use and category learning. Psychological Bulletin, 129(4), 592-613. Mix, K. S., Huttenlocher, J., & Levine, S. C. (2002). Multiple cues for quantification in infancy:is number one of them? Psychological Bulletin, 128(2), 278-294. Mix, K. S. (2008a). Children's numerical equivalence judgments:crossmapping effects. Cognitive Development, 23(1), 191-203. Mix, K. S. (2008b). Surface similarity and label knowledge impact early numerical comparisons. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 26(1), 13-32. Paik, J. H., & Mix, K. S. (2008). It's all relative:Different levels of relational similarity used in children's comparisons. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 26(4), 499-505. Piaget, J. (1997). The child's conception of number. London:Rout-ledge. (Original Work published 1941) Rattermann, M. J., & Gentner, D. (1998). The effect of language on similarity:The use of relational labels improves young children's performance in a mapping task. In K. Holyoak, D. Gentner, & B. Kokinov (Eds.), Advances in analogy research:Integration of theory and data from the cognitive, computational, and neural sciences (pp.274-282). Sofia:New Bulgarian University. Richland, L. E., Morrison, R. G., & Holyoak, K. J. (2006). Children's development of analogical reasoning:insights from scene analogy problems. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 94(3), 249-273. Sloutsky, V. M. (2003). The role of similarity in the development of categorization. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(6), 246-251. Sloutsky, V. M. (2010). From perceptual categories to concepts:what develops? Cognitive Science, 34(7), 1244-1286. Sloutsky, V. M. (2015). Conceptual Development. Handbook of Child Psychology and Developmental Science, 469-518 Sloutsky, V. M., Deng, W., Fisher, A. V., & Kloos, H. (2015). Conceptual influences on induction:a case for a late onset. Cognitive Psychology, 82, 1-31. Spelke, E. S. (2000). Core knowledge. American Psychologist, 55(11), 1233-1243. Wynn, K. (1995). Origins of numerical knowledge. Mathematical Cognition, 1, 35-60. Yuan, L., Uttal, D., & Gentner, D. (2017). Analogical processes in children's understanding of spatial representations. Developmental Psychology, 53(6), 1098-1114. 陈英和. (2015). 儿童数量表征与数概念的发展特点及机制. 心理发展与教育, 31(1), 21-28. 韩瑽瑽, 陈蒲晶, 陈英和. (2010). 儿童数概念发展的影响因素:表面相似性效应与标签效应. 心理发展与教育, 26(5), 449-456. 李红. (2015). 中国儿童推理能力发展的初步研究. 心理与行为研究, 13(5), 637-647. 李美佳, 庄丹琪, 彭华茂. (2018). 基于问题解决式的类比推理的老化:表面相似性和结构相似性的作用.心理学报, 50(11):1282-1291. 邱琴. (2012). 基于能力的类比推理映射转换的实验研究. 心理与行为研究, 10(6), 419-426. 辛自强. (2007). 关系-表征复杂性模型. 心理发展与教育, 23(3), 122-128. |
[1] | 郑显亮, 陈慧萍, 王雪, 鲍振宙. 青少年网络利他行为的发展趋势及社会阶层的影响:一项追踪研究[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(3): 333-341. |
[2] | 张玉平, 董琼, 宋爽, 舒华. 小学低年级儿童的阅读发展轨迹:早期语言认知技能的预测作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(2): 210-218. |
[3] | 张潮, 程亚华, 李利平, 范雨婷, 伍新春. 汉语儿童词语结构意识在阅读理解中的预测作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2022, 38(2): 236-243. |
[4] | 周婷娜, 李宜逊, 李虹, 徐钟庚, 张锋, 程亚华. 汉语儿童口语流畅性与阅读理解的关系:一项三年追踪研究[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2021, 37(5): 691-700. |
[5] | 吴鹏, 张琪, 王杨春子. 父母教养方式与初中生网络受欺负行为的关系:一项追踪研究[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2021, 37(5): 719-726. |
[6] | 陈子循, 冯映雪, 宋文莉, 刘霞. 亲子分离与青少年同伴侵害和抑郁的关系:一项追踪研究[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2021, 37(3): 429-438. |
[7] | 郑巧, 耿丽娜, 骆方, 李凌艳. 初中生学生投入的发展特点及其与同伴欺负的关系:一项三年追踪研究[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2020, 36(2): 157-167. |
[8] | 刘伟方, 张佳佳, 胡冬梅, 张明亮, 司继伟. 元认知监测与算术知识制约小学儿童心算策略运用能力的发展:一年纵向考察[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2019, 35(4): 439-446. |
[9] | 田媛, 宋友志, 周宗奎, 黄淳, 赵一倩. 青少年公正世界信念的发展轨迹及其影响因素:一项多水平分析[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2019, 35(2): 146-156. |
[10] | 方铖豪, 程亚华, 伍新春. 小学低年级儿童汉语语素意识、口语词汇知识对阅读能力的影响:一项追踪研究[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2019, 35(1): 57-67. |
[11] | 程亚华, 伍新春. 小学一年级阅读流畅性对二、三年级阅读理解的预测[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2018, 34(3): 314-321. |
[12] | 苏志强, 王钢, 刘传星, 张大均. 童年中晚期抑郁的发展及其与问题行为的并发:一项两年追踪研究[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2018, 34(2): 200-209. |
[13] | 钟萍, 邓慧华, 张光珍, 梁宗保, 陆祖宏. 青少年友谊关系质量与社交焦虑的相互影响:一项交叉滞后分析[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2017, 33(6): 727-733. |
[14] | 叶枝, 柴晓运, 郭海英, 翁欢欢, 林丹华. 流动性、教育安置方式和心理弹性对流动儿童孤独感的影响:一项追踪研究[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2017, 33(5): 595-604. |
[15] | 苏志强, 邵景进, 张大均, 蒲坚. 童年中晚期友谊质量与抑郁的关系:一项纵向研究[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2017, 33(4): 449-456. |
|