心理发展与教育 ›› 2020, Vol. 36 ›› Issue (5): 624-632.doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2020.05.14
王一伊1,2, 苏彦捷1,2
WANG Yiyi1,2, SU Yanjie1,2
摘要: 过度模仿指模仿与完成目标无关的动作。本文通过比较不同文化下儿童过度模仿行为的共性和差异,探究了儿童过度模仿可能的产生机制。不同文化下的儿童均存在过度模仿行为,说明它很可能是人类累积文明发展的重要产物;而不同文化下儿童过度模仿的倾向又存在差异,生存环境、教导式学习和对社会规范的强调等因素均可能产生影响。未来研究可考虑进一步探究不同文化因素的具体作用,并比较不同文化下个体过度模仿行为的发展进程,帮助我们更好地理解过度模仿在人类文明发展中的意义。
中图分类号:
Buchsbaum, D., Gopnik, A., Griffiths, T. L., & Shafto, P. (2011). Children's imitation of causal action sequences is influenced by statistical and pedagogical evidence. Cognition, 120(3), 331-340. Berl, R. E., & Hewlett, B. S. (2015). Cultural variation in the use of overimitation by the Aka and Ngandu of the Congo Basin. PLoS One, 10(3), e0120180. Brugger, A., Lariviere, L. A., Mumme, D. L., & Bushnell, E. W. (2007). Doing the right thing:Infants' selection of actions to imitate from observed event sequences. Child Development, 78(3), 806-824. Boyd, R., Richerson, P. J., & Henrich, J. (2011). The cultural niche:Why social learning is essential for human adaptation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108, 10918-10925. Corriveau, K. H., DiYanni, C. J., Clegg, J. M., Min, G., Chin, J., & Nasrini, J. (2017). Cultural differences in the imitation and transmission of inefficient actions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 161, 1-18. Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2011). Natural pedagogy as evolutionary adaptation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B:Biological Sciences, 366(1567), 1149-1157. Clegg, J. M., & Legare, C. H. (2016a). Instrumental and conventional interpretations of behavior are associated with distinct outcomes in early childhood. Child Development, 87(2), 527-542. Clegg, J. M., & Legare, C. H. (2016b). A cross-cultural comparison of children's imitative flexibility. Developmental Psychology, 52(9), 1435-1444. Chen, X., Liu, M., Li, B., Cen, G., Chen, H., & Wang, L. (2000). Maternal authoritative and authoritarian attitudes and mother-child interactions and relationships in urban China. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 24(1), 119-126. Clay, Z., Over, H., & Tennie, C. (2018). What drives young children to over-imitate? Investigating the effects of age, context, action type, and transitivity. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 166, 520-534. Clay, Z., & Tennie, C. (2018). Is overimitation a uniquely human phenomenon? Insights from human children as compared to bonobos. Child Development, 89(5), 1535-1544. Clegg, J. M., Wen, N., & Legare, C. H. (2017). Is non-conformity WEIRD? Cultural variation in adults' beliefs about children's competency and conformity. Journal of Experimental Psychology:General, 146(3), 428-441. DiYanni, C. J., Corriveau, K. H., Kurkul, K., Nasrini, J., & Nini, D. (2015). The role of consensus and culture in children's imitation of inefficient actions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 137, 99-110. Flynn, E. (2008). Investigating children as cultural magnets:Do young children transmit redundant information along diffusion chains? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B:Biological Sciences, 363(1509), 3541-3551. Frick, A., Clément, F., & Gruber, T. (2017). Evidence for a sex effect during overimitation:Boys copy irrelevant modelled actions more than girls across cultures. Royal Society Open Science, 4(12), 170367. Freier, L., Cooper, R. P., & Mareschal, D. (2015). The planning and execution of natural sequential actions in the preschool years. Cognition, 144, 58-66. Flynn, E., & Smith, K. (2012). Investigating the mechanisms of cultural acquisition. Social Psychology, 43(4), 185-195. Gaskins, S. (2006). Cultural perspectives on infant-caregiver interaction. In S. Levenson & N. Enfield (Eds.), Roots of human sociality:Culture, cognition, and human interaction (pp. 279-298). Oxford, England:Berg. Gellén, K., & Buttelmann, D. (2019). Rational imitation declines within the second year of life:Changes in the function of imitation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 185, 148-163. Gruber, T., Deschenaux, A., Frick, A., & Clément, F. (2019). Group membership influences more social identification than social learning or overimitation in children. Child Development, 90(3), 728-745. Greenfield, P. M., Keller, H., Fuligni, A., & Maynard, A. (2003). Cultural pathways through universal development. Annual Review of Psychology, 54(1), 461-490. Hofstede, G. (2001). Culture's consequences:Comparing values, behaviors, institutions, and organizations across nations (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA:Sage. Hewlett, B. S., Fouts, H. N., Boyette, A. H., & Hewlett, B. L. (2011). Social learning among Congo Basin hunter-gatherers. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B:Biological Sciences, 366(1567), 1168-1178. Hoehl, S., Keupp, S., Schleihauf, H., McGuigan, N., Buttelmann, D., & Whiten, A. (2019). ‘Over-imitation’:A review and appraisal of a decade of research. Developmental Review, 51, 90-108. Huber, L., Popovová, N., Riener, S., Salobir, K., & Cimarelli, G. (2018). Would dogs copy irrelevant actions from their human caregiver? Learning & Behavior, 46(4), 387-397. Hilbrink, E. E., Sakkalou, E., Ellis-Davies, K., Fowler, N. C., & Gattis, M. (2013). Selective and faithful imitation at 12 and 15 months. Developmental Science, 16(6), 828-840. Horner, V., & Whiten, A. (2005). Causal knowledge and imitation/emulation switching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children (Homo sapiens). Animal Cognition, 8(3), 164-181. Kenward, B. (2012). Over-imitating preschoolers believe unnecessary actions are normative and enforce their performance by a third party. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 112(2), 195-207. Keupp, S., Behne, T., & Rakoczy, H. (2013). Why do children overimitate? Normativity is crucial. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116(2), 392-406. Keupp, S., Behne, T., & Rakoczy, H. (2018). The Rationality of (Over)imitation. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 13(6), 678-687. Keupp, S., Bancken, C., Schillmöller, J., Rakoczy, H., & Behne, T. (2016). Rational over-imitation:Preschoolers consider material costs and copy causally irrelevant actions selectively. Cognition, 147, 85-92. Keupp, S., Behne, T., Zachow, J., Kasbohm, A., & Rakoczy, H. (2015). Over-imitation is not automatic:Context sensitivity in children's overimitation and action interpretation of causally irrelevant actions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 130, 163-175. Kenward, B., Karlsson, M., & Persson, J. (2010). Over-imitation is better explained by norm learning than by distorted causal learning. Proceedings of the Royal Society B:Biological Sciences, 278(1709), 1239-1246. Lycett, S. J. (2008). Acheulean variation and selection:Does handaxe symmetry fit neutral expectations? Journal of Archaeological Science, 35(9), 2640-2648. Lyons, D. E., Damrosch, D. H., Lin, J. K., Macris, D. M., & Keil, F. C. (2011). The scope and limits of overimitation in the transmission of artefact culture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B:Biological Sciences, 366(1567), 1158-1167. Legare, C. H., & Nielsen, M. (2015). Imitation and innovation:The dual engines of cultural learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(11), 688-699. Lew-Levy, S., Reckin, R., Lavi, N., Cristóbal-Azkarate, J., & Ellis-Davies, K. (2017). How do hunter-gatherer children learn subsistence skills? Human Nature, 28(4), 367-394. Lyons, D. E., Young, A. G., & Keil, F. C. (2007). The hidden structure of overimitation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(50), 19751-19756. Marlowe, F. W. (2005). Hunter-gatherers and human evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology:Issues, News, and Reviews, 14(2), 54-67. McGuigan, N. (2012). The role of transmission biases in the cultural diffusion of irrelevant actions. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126(2), 150. McGuigan, N. (2013). The influence of model status on the tendency of young children to over-imitate. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116(4), 962-969. McGuigan, N., Gladstone, D., & Cook, L. (2012). Is the cultural transmission of irrelevant tool actions in adult humans (Homo sapiens) best explained as the result of an evolved conformist bias? PLoS One, 7(12), e50863. Machin, A. J., Hosfield, R. T., & Mithen, S. J. (2007). Why are some handaxes symmetrical? Testing the influence of handaxe morphology on butchery effectiveness. Journal of Archaeological Science, 34(6), 883-893. Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self:Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98(2), 224. Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1989). Imitation in newborn infants:Exploring the range of gestures imitated and the underlying mechanisms. Developmental Psychology, 25(6), 954. McGuigan, N., Makinson, J., & Whiten, A. (2011). From over-imitation to super-copying:Adults imitate causally irrelevant aspects of tool use with higher fidelity than young children. British Journal of Psychology, 102(1), 1-18. Marsh, L. E., Ropar, D., & Hamilton, A. F. D. C. (2014). The social modulation of imitation fidelity in school-age children. PLoS One, 9(1), e86127. McGuigan, N., & Whiten, A. (2009). Emulation and "overemulation" in the social learning of causally opaque versus causally transparent tool use by 23-and 30-month-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 104(4), 367-381. McGuigan, N., Whiten, A., Flynn, E., & Horner, V. (2007). Imitation of causally opaque versus causally transparent tool use by 3-and 5-year-old children. Cognitive Development, 22(3), 353-364. Nielsen, M. (2006). Copying actions and copying outcomes:Social learning through the second year. Developmental Psychology, 42(3), 555-565. Nielsen, M. (2018). The social glue of cumulative culture and ritual behavior. Child Development Perspectives, 12(4), 264-268. Nielsen, M., & Blank, C. (2011). Imitation in young children:When who gets copied is more important than what gets copied. Developmental Psychology, 47(4), 1050-1053. Nielsen, M., Cucchiaro, J., & Mohamedally, J. (2012). When the transmission of culture is child's play. PLoS One, 7(3), e34066. Nielsen, M., Moore, C., & Mohamedally, J. (2012). Young children overimitate in third-party contexts. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 112(1), 73-83. Nielsen, M., Mushin, I., Tomaselli, K., & Whiten, A. (2014). Where culture takes hold:"Overimitation" and its flexible deployment in Western, Aboriginal, and Bushmen children. Child Development, 85(6), 2169-2184. Nielsen, M., Mushin, I., Tomaselli, K., & Whiten, A. (2016). Imitation, collaboration, and their interaction among Western and Indigenous Australian preschool children. Child Development, 87(3), 795-806. Nielsen, M., & Tomaselli, K. (2010). Overimitation in Kalahari Bushman children and the origins of human cultural cognition. Psychological Science, 21(5), 729-736. Over, H., & Carpenter, M. (2012). Putting the social into social learning:Explaining both selectivity and fidelity in children's copying behavior. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126(2), 182-192. Over, H., & Carpenter, M. (2013). The social side of imitation. Child Development Perspectives, 7(1), 6-11. Oyserman, D., & Lee, S. W. (2008). Does culture influence what and how we think? Effects of priming individualism and collectivism. Psychological Bulletin, 134(2), 311. Rendell, L., Boyd, R., Cownden, D., Enquist, M., Eriksson, K., Feldman, M. W., et al. (2010). Why copy others? Insights from the social learning strategies tournament. Science, 328(5975), 208-213. Schachner, A., & Carey, S. (2013). Reasoning about ‘irrational’ actions:When intentional movements cannot be explained, the movements themselves are seen as the goal. Cognition, 129(2), 309-327. Strachan, J., Samuel, J., & Takaro, M. (2007). Ni Vanuatu women graduates:What happens when they go home? Development in Practice, 17(1), 147-153. Triandis, H. C., & Gelfand, M. J. (1998). Converging measurement of horizontal and vertical individualism and collectivism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(1), 118-128. Taniguchi, Y., & Sanefuji, W. (2017). The boundaries of overimitation in preschool children:Effects of target and tool use on imitation of irrelevant actions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 159, 83-95. Whiten, A., Allan, G., Devlin, S., Kseib, N., Raw, N., & McGuigan, N. (2016). Social learning in the real-world:'Over-imitation'occurs in both children and adults unaware of participation in an experiment and independently of social interaction. PLoS One, 11(7), e0159920. Wang, X. L., Bernas, R., & Eberhard, P. (2008). Responding to children's everyday transgressions in Chinese working-class families. Journal of Moral Education, 37(1), 55-79. Watson-Jones, R. E., Legare, C. H., Whitehouse, H., & Clegg, J. M. (2014). Task-specific effects of ostracism on imitative fidelity in early childhood. Evolution and Human Behavior, 35(3), 204-210. Whiten, A., McGuigan, N., Marshall-Pescini, S., & Hopper, L. M. (2009). Emulation, imitation, over-imitation and the scope of culture for child and chimpanzee. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B:Biological Sciences, 364(1528), 2417-2428. Wang, Q., & Su, Y. J. (2006). Myth of the most frequently used cultural frames:A critical look at individualism/collectivism and independent/interdependent self-construals. Acta Scientiarum Naturalium Universitatis Pekinensis, 42(2), 159-167. Xu, Y., Farver, J. A. M., Zhang, Z., Zeng, Q., Yu, L., & Cai, B. (2005). Mainland Chinese parenting styles and parent-child interaction. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 29(6), 524-531. Yu, Y., Bonawitz, E., & Shafto, P. (2019). Pedagogical questions in parent-child conversations. Child Development, 90(1), 147-161. 夏彧婷, 陈英和. (2016). 儿童过度模仿的原因, 意义及其教育启示. 学前教育研究, 1, 49-56. 夏彧婷, 胡清芬. (2015). 遵守规范的动机对儿童过度模仿的影响. 心理科学, 38(3), 605-611. 张新, 宮进才, 陆玉林. (1998). 再塑人文. 北京:中国经济出版社. |
[1] | 储月, 刘希平, 徐慧, 唐卫海. 儿童社会分享型提取诱发遗忘的发展特点[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(2): 153-159. |
[2] | 刘艳春, 邓玉婷, 张曦. 智力障碍儿童对不同对象的分享行为:心理理论的作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(2): 160-168. |
[3] | 梁丹丹, 闫晓民, 葛志林. 4~8岁汉语高功能自闭症儿童基于语言线索的情绪识别能力发展研究[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(2): 169-175. |
[4] | 史梦梦, 任桂琴, 孙军红, 张鑫星. 词汇类型和阅读水平对小学一年级儿童阅读理解监控的影响[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(2): 207-214. |
[5] | 冷欣怡, 苏萌萌, 李文玲, 杨秀杰, 邢爱玲, 张湘琳, 舒华. 家庭环境与农村儿童早期语言发展的关系[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(1): 8-18. |
[6] | 赵京伟, 陈晓旭, 任立文, 耿喆, 徐夫真. 父母心理控制与小学儿童焦虑:一个有调节的中介模型[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(1): 93-102. |
[7] | 樊香麟, 崔英锦. 客体化身体意识与女大学生限制性饮食行为的关系:外貌负面评价恐惧和社会文化压力的中介作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(1): 122-131. |
[8] | 朱娜平, 刘雁伶, 熊红梅, 赵攀. “公无远近”有早晚:不同资源分配情境中儿童公平规范执行行为发展特点[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(6): 772-780. |
[9] | 颜志强, 周可, 曾晓, 徐惠, 朱晓倩, 张娟. 学前期儿童执行功能与攻击性行为的关系:认知共情的中介作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(6): 788-797. |
[10] | 任立文, 马原驰, 张清瑶, 张玲玲, 徐夫真. 父母心理控制与儿童学校适应的关系:基于交叉滞后的分析[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(5): 635-644. |
[11] | 李甜甜, 董会芹. 父母冲突知觉与儿童焦虑情绪:正负性信息注意偏向的中介作用及性别的调节作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(4): 488-496. |
[12] | 银小兰, 周路军, 朱翠英. 学校环境对农村留守儿童亲社会行为的影响:心理资本与生活满意度的中介作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(4): 497-504. |
[13] | 张文新. 欺凌的界定:文化和发展的视角[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(4): 590-598. |
[14] | Peter K. Smith. 校园欺凌的跨文化差异问题:特征与干预[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(4): 599-608. |
[15] | 赵纤, 王志航, 王东方, 袁言云, 尹霞云, 黎志华. 贫困家庭儿童在青少年早期的亲社会行为发展轨迹:性别及父母教养方式异质性的影响[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(3): 323-332. |
|