心理发展与教育 ›› 2020, Vol. 36 ›› Issue (1): 10-18.doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2020.01.02
官冬晓, 艾继如, 黄碧娟, 崔爽, 司继伟
GUAN Dongxiao, AI Jiru, HUANG Bijuan, CUI Shuang, SI Jiwei
摘要: 为了考察数学焦虑对儿童数量表征表现的可能影响及认知抑制的潜在调节作用,选取70名小学三年级儿童(高焦虑组36人,低焦虑组34人)为被试,在对抑制条件进行操控的情况下,要求其完成符号、非符号数量表征任务。结果发现,被试在两种数量表征任务中均出现距离效应,与符号数量比较任务相比,高焦虑组在非符号数量比较任务中的正确率显著低于低焦虑组,且高焦虑组表现出了更强的距离效应。鉴于非符号数量比较任务更能反映出个体近似数量系统(ANS)的敏锐性,上述结果意味着高数学焦虑儿童的数量表征更不精确,其在相对复杂问题上较差的表现或许源于基本数量能力缺陷。本研究还发现认知抑制能够调节数学焦虑对个体非符号数量表征的影响,抑制条件下高低焦虑组儿童在正确率指标上的差异大于非抑制条件,抑制条件的设置提高了个体对工作记忆资源的需求,此时焦虑情绪对认知资源的消耗会造成任务所需资源的不足,进而削弱高焦虑个体的认知效用。
Ashcraft, M. H. (2002). Math anxiety:Personal, educational, and cognitive consequences. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11(5), 181-185. Ashcraft, M. H., & Faust, M. W. (1994). Mathematics anxiety and mental arithmetic performance:An exploratory investigation. Cognition & Emotion, 8(2), 97-125. Ashcraft, M. H., & Kirk, E. P. (2001). The relationships among working memory, math anxiety, and performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology:General, 130(2), 224-37. Ashcraft, M. H., & Moore, A. M. (2009). Mathematics anxiety and the affective drop in performance. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 27(3), 197-205. Krajcsi, A., Lengyel, G., & Kojouharova, P. (2018). Symbolic number comparison is not processed by the analog number system:Different symbolic and non-symbolic numerical distance and size effects. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 124. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00124 Bonny, J. W., & Lourenco, S. F. (2013). The approximate number system and its relation to early math achievement:Evidence from the preschool years. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114(3), 375-388. Brysbaert, M. (2005). Number recognition in different formats. In Campbell, J. I. D. (Ed.), Handbook of Mathematical Cognition (pp. 23-42). New York:Psychology Press. Cantlon. J. F., Platt, M. L., & Brannon, E. M. (2009). Beyond the number domain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(2), 83-91. Cargnelutti, E., Tomasetto, C., & Passolunghi, M. C. (2016). How is anxiety related to math performance in young students? A longitudinal study of grade 2 to grade 3 children. Cognition & Emotion, 31(4), 755-764. De Smedt. B., Verschaffel, L., & Ghesquière, P. (2009). The predictive value of numerical magnitude comparison for individual differences in mathematics achievement. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103(4), 469-479. Dehaene, S. (1997). The number sense. New York:Oxford University Press. Dehaene, S., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., & Cohen, L. (1998). Abstract representations of numbers in the animal and human brain. Trends in Neuroscience, 21(8), 355-361. Dehaene, S., Piazza, M., Pinel, P., & Cohen, L. (2003). Three parietal circuits for number processing. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20(3), 487-506. Dietrich, J. F., Huber, S., Moeller, K., & Klein, E. (2015). The influence of math anxiety on symbolic and non-symbolic magnitude processing.Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1621. Doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01621 Eysenck, M. W., & Calvo, M. G. (1992). Anxiety and performance:the processing efficiency theory. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 409-434. Eysenck, M. W., Derakshan, N., Santos, R., & Calvo, M. G. (2007). Anxiety and cognitive performance:Attentional control theory. Emotion, 7(2), 336-353. Fernanda, D. O. F., Wood, G., Pinheirochagas, P., Lonnemann, J., Krinzinger, H., Willmes, K., et al. (2012). Explaining school mathematics performance from symbolic and nonsymbolic magnitude processing:Similarities and differences between typical and low-achieving children. Psychology & Neuroscience, 5(1), 37-4606. French, J. W., Ekstrom, R. B., & Price, L. A. (1963). Kit of reference tests for cognitive factors. Princeton, N.J:Educational Testing Service. Fuhs, M. W., & McNeil, N. M. (2013). ANS acuity and mathematics ability in preschoolers from low-income homes:Contributions of inhibitory control. Developmental Science, 16(1), 136-148. Geary, D. C., & Vanmarle, K. (2016). Young children's core symbolic and nonsymbolic quantitative knowledge in the prediction of later mathematics achievement. Developmental Psychology, 52(12), 2130-2144. Gilmore, C., Attridge, N., Clayton, S., Cragg, L., Johnson, S., Marlow, N., et al. (2013). Individual differences in inhibitory control, not non-verbal number acuity, correlate with mathematics achievement. Plos One, 8(6), e67374. DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0067374 Gilmore, C., Keeble, S., Richardson, S., & Cragg, L. (2015). The role of cognitive inhibition in different components of arithmetic. ZDM, 47(5), 771-782. Halberda, J., & Feigenson, L. (2008). Developmental change in the acuity of the "number sense":the approximate number system in 3-, 4-, 5-, and 6-year-olds and adults. Developmental Psychology, 44(5), 1457-1465. Holloway, I. D., & Ansari, D. (2009). Mapping numerical magnitudes onto symbols:The numerical distance effect and individual differences in children's mathematics achievement. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103(1), 17-29. Imbo, I. (2007). Strategy selection and strategy efficiency in mental arithmetic (Doctoral Dissertation). Gent University. Landerl, Karin, Bevan, Anna, Butterworth, & Brian. (2004). Developmental dyscalculia and basic numerical capacities:A study of 8-9-year-old students. Cognition, 93(2), 99-125. Lindskog, M., Winman, A., & Poom, L. (2017). Individual differences in nonverbal number skills predict math anxiety. Cognition, 159, 156-162. Lonnemann, J., Linkersdörfer, J., Hasselhorn, M., & Lindberg, S. (2011). Symbolic and non-symbolic distance effects in children and their connection with arithmetic skills. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 24(5), 583-591. Lyons, I. M., Ansari, D., & Beilock, S. L. (2015). Qualitatively different coding of symbolic and non-symbolic numbers in the human brain. Human Brain Mapping. 36, 475-488. Maloney, E. A., Ansari, D., & Fugelsang, J. A. (2011). The effect of mathematics anxiety on the processing of numerical magnitude. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(1), 10-16. Maloney, E. A., Risko, E. F., Ansari, D., & Fugelsang, J. (2010). Mathematics anxiety affects counting but not subitizing during visual enumeration. Cognition, 114(2), 293-297. Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive function and their contribution to complex "Frontal Lobe" tasks:A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49-100. Mock, J., Huber, S, Klein, E., & Moeller, K. (2016). Insights into numerical cognition:considering eye-fixations in number processing and arithmetic. Psychological Research, 80(3), 334-359. Moyer, R. S., & Landauer, T. K. (1967). Time required for judgments of numerical inequality. Nature, 215(5190), 1519-1520. Núñez-Peña, M. I., & Suárez-Pellicioni, M. (2014). Less precise representation of numerical magnitude in high math-anxious individuals:An ERP study of the size and distance effects. Biological Psychology, 103, 176-183. Pizzie, R. G., & Kraemer, D. J. M. (2017). Avoiding math on a rapid timescale:Emotional responsivity and anxious attention in math anxiety. Brain and Cognition, 118, 100-107. Ramirez, G., Chang, H., Maloney, E. A., Levine, S. C., & Beilock, S. L. (2016). On the relationship between math anxiety and math achievement in early elementary school:The role of problem solving strategies. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 141, 83-100. Ramirez, G., Gunderson, E, A., Levine, S, C., & Beilock. S, L. (2013). Math anxiety, working memory, and math achievement in early elementary school. Journal of Cognition & Development, 14(2), 187-202. Sabourin, J. L., & Lester, J. C. (2014). Affect and engagement in game-based learning environments. IEEE Transactions on Affective Computing, 5(1), 45-56. Siegler, R. S., & Pyke, A. A. (2013). Developmental and individual differences in understanding of fractions. Developmental Psychology, 49(10), 1994-2004. Szucs, D., Devine, A., Soltesz, F., Nobes, A., & Gabriel, F. (2013). Developmental dyscalculia is related to visuo-spatial memory and inhibition impairment. Cortex, 49(10), 2674-2688. Thomas, G., & Dowker, A. (2000). Mathematics anxiety and related factors in young children. Paper presented at British Psychological Society Developmental Section Conference, Bristol, UK. Titia, G., & Bert, R. (2011). Generating non-symbolic number stimuli. Behavior Research Methods, 43(4), 981-986 陈琪. (2016). 数学焦虑影响数学任务表现的作用机制研究(硕士学位论文). 内蒙古师范大学, 呼和浩特. 陈英和. (2015). 儿童数量表征与数概念的发展特点及机制. 心理发展与教育, 31(1), 21-28. 陈英和, 耿柳娜. (2005). 数学焦虑对儿童加减法认知策略选择和执行的影响. 心理发展与教育, 21(4), 24-27. 陈英和, 赖颖慧. (2013). 儿童非符号数量表征的特点及作用探析. 北京师范大学学报(社会科学版),21(1), 33-41. 黄碧娟. (2017). 算术技能与数学焦虑的关系及其对数字认知的影响:行为和电生理学证据(硕士学位论文). 山东师范大学,济南. 李红霞, 司继伟, 陈泽建, 张堂正. (2015). 人类的近似数量系统. 心理科学进展, 23(4), 562-570. 刘桂荣, 张景焕. (2017). 教育心理学研究的特点和趋势-基于2011-2015年Journal of Educational Psychology刊载文章的分析. 山东师范大学学报, 62(4), 123-132. 牛玉柏, 张丽芬, 肖帅, 曹贤才. (2018). 小学生近似数量系统敏锐度的发展趋势及其与数学能力的关系:抑制控制的中介作用. 心理科学, 41(2), 344-350. 司继伟, 徐艳丽, 封洪敏, 许晓华, 周超. (2014). 不同数学焦虑成人的算术策略运用差异:ERP研究. 心理学报, 46(12), 1835-1849. 孙燕, 司继伟, 徐艳丽. (2012). 数学焦虑影响大学生/儿童估算策略运用的对比研究. 心理发展与教育, 28(3), 263-270. 腾静. (2015). 儿童非符号数量表征与数学学业成绩的关系(硕士学位论文). 华东师范大学, 上海. 王翠艳, 刘昌. (2007). 数学焦虑对心算加工的影响. 心理科学进展, 15(5), 795-801. 王君, 陈天勇. (2012). 抑制控制与高级认知功能的关系. 心理科学进展, 20(11), 1768-1778. 闫国利, 熊建萍, 臧传丽, 余莉莉, 崔磊, 白学军. (2013). 阅读研究中的主要眼动指标评述. 心理科学进展, 21(4), 589-605. |
[1] | 储月, 刘希平, 徐慧, 唐卫海. 儿童社会分享型提取诱发遗忘的发展特点[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(2): 153-159. |
[2] | 刘艳春, 邓玉婷, 张曦. 智力障碍儿童对不同对象的分享行为:心理理论的作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(2): 160-168. |
[3] | 梁丹丹, 闫晓民, 葛志林. 4~8岁汉语高功能自闭症儿童基于语言线索的情绪识别能力发展研究[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(2): 169-175. |
[4] | 史梦梦, 任桂琴, 孙军红, 张鑫星. 词汇类型和阅读水平对小学一年级儿童阅读理解监控的影响[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(2): 207-214. |
[5] | 冷欣怡, 苏萌萌, 李文玲, 杨秀杰, 邢爱玲, 张湘琳, 舒华. 家庭环境与农村儿童早期语言发展的关系[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(1): 8-18. |
[6] | 赵京伟, 陈晓旭, 任立文, 耿喆, 徐夫真. 父母心理控制与小学儿童焦虑:一个有调节的中介模型[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(1): 93-102. |
[7] | 朱娜平, 刘雁伶, 熊红梅, 赵攀. “公无远近”有早晚:不同资源分配情境中儿童公平规范执行行为发展特点[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(6): 772-780. |
[8] | 颜志强, 周可, 曾晓, 徐惠, 朱晓倩, 张娟. 学前期儿童执行功能与攻击性行为的关系:认知共情的中介作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(6): 788-797. |
[9] | 任立文, 马原驰, 张清瑶, 张玲玲, 徐夫真. 父母心理控制与儿童学校适应的关系:基于交叉滞后的分析[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(5): 635-644. |
[10] | 李甜甜, 董会芹. 父母冲突知觉与儿童焦虑情绪:正负性信息注意偏向的中介作用及性别的调节作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(4): 488-496. |
[11] | 银小兰, 周路军, 朱翠英. 学校环境对农村留守儿童亲社会行为的影响:心理资本与生活满意度的中介作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(4): 497-504. |
[12] | 赵纤, 王志航, 王东方, 袁言云, 尹霞云, 黎志华. 贫困家庭儿童在青少年早期的亲社会行为发展轨迹:性别及父母教养方式异质性的影响[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(3): 323-332. |
[13] | 张玉平, 董琼, 宋爽, 舒华. 小学低年级儿童的阅读发展轨迹:早期语言认知技能的预测作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(2): 210-218. |
[14] | 李君, 王悦, 陈夏妮, 李莹. 二语学习对汉语学龄儿童认知控制与词汇通达的作用——年龄和二语熟练程度的影响[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(2): 219-227. |
[15] | 鲁明辉, 王融, 张丽敏. 父母心理弹性与自闭症谱系障碍儿童情绪行为问题的关系:一个有调节的中介模型[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(2): 247-254. |
|