Psychological Development and Education ›› 2021, Vol. 37 ›› Issue (1): 68-75.doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2021.01.09
Previous Articles Next Articles
GU Chanyuan1,2, BI Hongyan1,2
CLC Number:
Andrews, S. (1997). The effect of orthographic similarity on lexical retrieval:Resolving neighborhood conflicts.Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4(4), 439-461. Andrews, S. (1989). Frequency and Neighborhood Effects on Lexical Access:Activation or Search? Journal of Experimental Psychology:Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15(5), 802-814. Cai, Q., & Brysbaert, M. (2010). SUBTLEX-CH:Chinese Word and Character Frequencies Based on Film Subtitles.PLOS ONE, 5(6), Article e10729.http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0010729 Carreiras, M., Perea, M., & Grainger, J. (1997). Effects of orthographic neighborhood in visual word recognition:Cross-task comparisons.Journal of Experimental Psychology:Learning Memory and Cognition, 23(4), 857-871. Castles, A, Davis, C., & Letcher, T. (1999). Neighbourhood effects on masked form priming in developing readers.Language and Cognitive Processes, 14(2), 201-224. Castles, A., Davis, C., Cavalot, P., & Forster, K. (2007). Tracking the acquisition of orthographic skills in developing readers:Masked priming effects.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 97(3), 165-182. Charles-Luce, J., & Luce, P. A. (1990). Similarity neighborhoods of words in young children's lexicons.Journal of Child Language, 17, 205-215. Coady, J. A., & Aslin, R. N.(2003). Phonological neighbourhoods in the developing lexicon.Journal of Child Language, 30(2), 441-469. Coltheart, M., Davelaar, E., Jonasson, J. T., & Besner, D. (1977). Access to the internal lexicon. In S. Dornic (Ed.), Attention and performance VI (pp. 535-555). Hillsdale, NJ:Eribaum. Dandurand, F., Grainger, J., Duñabeitia, J. A., & Granier, J.-P. (2011). On Coding Non-Contiguous Letter Combinations.Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 136. Dollaghan, C. A. (1994).Children's Phonological Neighborhoods:Half Empty of Half Full?. Journal of Child Language, 21(2), 257-271. Duñabeitia, J. A., & Vidal-Abarca, E. (2008). Children Like Dense Neighborhoods:Orthographic Neighborhood Density Effects in Novel Readers.The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 11(1), 26-35. Grainger, J.(1990). Word frequency and neighborhood frequency effects in lexical decision and naming.Journal of Memory and Language, 29(2), 228-244. Grainger, J., Lété, B., Bertrand, D., Dufau, S., & Ziegler, J. C. (2012). Evidence for multiple routes in learning to read.Cognition, 123(2), 280-292. Laxon, V. J., Coltheart, V., & Keating, C. (1988). Children find friendly words friendly too:Words with many orthographic neighbours are easier to read and spell.British Journal of Educational Psychology, 58(1), 103-119. Li, M.-F., Lin, W.-C., Chou, T.-L., Yang, F.-L., & Wu, J.-T. (2015). The Role of Orthographic Neighborhood Size Effects in Chinese Word Recognition.Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 44(3), 219-236. Li, Q.L., Bi, H.Y., Wei, T.Q., & Chen, B.G. (2011). Orthographic neighborhood size effect in Chinese character naming:Orthographic and phonological activations.Acta Psychologica, 136(1), 35-41. Marinus, E., & deJong, P.F. (2010). Size does not matter, frequency does:Sensitivity to orthographic neighbors in normal and dyslexic readers.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 106, 129-144. Mcclelland, J.L., & Rumelhart, D.E. (1981). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception:I. An account of basic findings.Psychological Review, 88(5), 375-407. Sears, C.R., Hino, Y., & Lupker, S.J. (1995). Neighborhood size and neighborhood frequency effects in word recognition.Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception & Performance, 21(4), 876-900. Yao, Y. (2009). Phonological Neighbourhood Development in Children's Lexicons. Uc Berkeley Phonology Lab Annual Report, 44-63. Zhao, J., Li, Q.L., & Bi, H.Y. (2012). The Characteristics of Chinese Orthographic Neighborhood Size Effect for Developing Readers.PloS One, 7(10), Article e46922.https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.0046922 Zhao, J., Li, Q.L., Ding, G.S., & Bi, H.Y. (2015). Development of neural basis for chinese orthographic neighborhood size effect.Human Brain Mapping, 37(2), 632-647. Ziegler, J. C., Bertrand, D., Lete, B., & Grainger, J. (2014). Orthographic and Phonological Contributions to Reading Development:Tracking Developmental Trajectories Using Masked Priming.Developmental Psychology, 50(4), 1026-1036. 毕鸿燕, 胡伟, 翁旭初. (2006). 汉语形声字声旁家族大小对整字发音的影响. 心理学报, 38(6), 791-797. 姜敏敏,张积家,李虎. (2011). 声旁家族大小和高频同声旁字对汉字命名的影响. 心理学探新, 31(5), 434-439. 李燕, 康加深. (1993). 现代汉语形声字声符研究. 见陈原(编). 现代汉语用字信息分析(pp. 84-98). 上海:上海教育出版社. 钱怡, 张逸玮, 毕鸿燕. (2015). 汉字识别中的形旁家族效应. 人类工效学, 21(3), 25-30. 舒华, 曾红梅. (1996). 儿童对汉字结构中语音线索的意识及其发展. 心理学报, 28(2), 160-165. 温红博, 唐文君, 刘先伟. (2015). 义务教育阶段学生识字量测验的编制研究. 语言文字应用, (3), 88-100. 周有光. (1980). 汉字声旁读音便查. 长春:吉林人民出版社. |
|