心理发展与教育 ›› 2018, Vol. 34 ›› Issue (6): 641-648.doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2018.06.01

• 认知与社会性发展 •    下一篇

“我的”最重要:儿童自我源判断能力的发展早于他人

王海旭1, 刘明慧2, 叶宇坤3, 隋洁4   

  1. 1. 清华大学心理学系, 北京 100084;
    2. 哈尔滨师范大学教育科学学院, 哈尔滨 150025;
    3. 万花筒幼儿园, 哈尔滨 150078;
    4. 巴斯大学心理学系, 英国, BA2 7AY
  • 出版日期:2018-11-15 发布日期:2018-12-27
  • 通讯作者: 刘明慧,E-mail:liumh827@126.com E-mail:liumh827@126.com
  • 基金资助:
    国家自然科学基金(31471001);黑龙江省哲学社会科学研究规划年度项目(17SHC130);中国积极心理学研究基金(京民基证字第0020344号2015-01-033)。

“Mine” is the Most Important: the Development of Self Source -Judgment for Children is Earlier than Other's

WANG Haixu1, LIU Minghui2, YE Yukun3, SUI Jie4   

  1. 1. Department of Psychology, Tsinghua University, Beijing 100084;
    2. School of Education, Harbin Normal University, Harbin 150025;
    3. Wanhuatong Kindergarten, Harbin 150078;
    4. Department of Psychology, University of Bath, Bath BA2 7AY, UK
  • Online:2018-11-15 Published:2018-12-27

摘要: 自我参照范式是探索自我认知发展的重要范式,找到一个可靠方法获得一份稳定结果一直是儿童研究的重点和难点。研究以3岁至5岁儿童作为研究对象,通过所有权参照进行编码、图片再认和源判断进行检测。为了结果的稳定可靠,每位儿童需要参加4次测验。结果显示,图片再认上4岁、5岁儿童出现自我参照效应,源判断上3岁、4岁儿童出现自我参照效应。对源判断结果分析发现,自我参照成绩在各年龄组间保持不变、他人参照成绩随年龄增长逐渐提高,二者发展趋势的差异是5岁儿童源判断上没有自我参照效应的主要原因。以上结果说明,3~5岁儿童普遍存在自我参照效应,但测验方法会影响该效应的表现年龄;其次,自我源判断能力的发展早于他人,3~5岁之间他人源判断能力是在持续提高的;另外,4次测验间的比较证明,源判断测验较之再认具有更好的稳定性。

关键词: 儿童, 自我参照记忆, 所有权, 源判断

Abstract: The concept of the self has been used to interpret how we interact with the world and think about both our own behavior and the behavior of others. A key but controversial question is when self-concept development begins. However, direct evidence of this question is limited due to different tasks in the studies of self-reference effect (SRE). As a result, identifying a suitable method for developmental research in the field of self is vital. This was the aim of the present study.We tested three age groups (3, 4 and 5 year olds) in a SRE task in which ownership-referential judgments were used during encoding stage and then recognition and source judgments were underwent. There were four testing sessions within individuals in order to examine the stability of test-retest, and each two sessions took place one week apart.The results showed that 4-and 5-year-old children had a SRE in recognition, while 3-and 4-year-old children demonstrated the SRE in source memory. Follow-up analyses further revealed that the source memory maintained in the self-condition across three age groups but their performance in the other condition enhanced with age. These results indicate that the self-concept may emerge earlier than the concept of others. The implications of these findings are discussed.

Key words: children, self-referential effect in memory, ownership, source memory

中图分类号: 

  • B844
Cacioppo, J. T., & Petty, R. E. (1979). Effects of message repetition and position on cognitive response, recall, and persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37(1), 97-109.
Cunningham, S. J., Brebner, J. L., Quinn, F., & Turk, D. J. (2014). The self-reference effect on memory in early childhood. Child Development, 85(2), 808-823.
Cunningham, S. J., Turk, D. J., Macdonald, L. M., & Macrae, C. N. (2008). Yours or mine? Ownership and memory. Consciousness and Cognition, 17(1), 312-318.
Cunningham, S. J., Vergunst, F., Macrae, C. N., & Turk, D. J. (2013). Exploring early self-referential memory effects through ownership. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 31(3), 289-301.
Fasig, L. G. (2000). Toddlers' understanding of ownership:Implications for self-concept development. Social Development, 9(3), 370-382.
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A., & Buchner, A. (2007). G Power 3:A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175-191.
Foley, M. A., & Johnson, M. K. (1985). Confusions between memories for performed and imagined actions:A developmental comparison. Child Development, 56, 1145-1155.
Foley, M. A., Passalacqua, C., & Ratner, H. H. (1993). Appropriating the actions of another:Implications for children's memory and learning. Cognitive Development, 8(4), 373-401.
Hala, S., Brown, A. M., McKay, L., & San Juan, V. (2013). Two-and-a-half-year-olds' memory for sources of actions:Contextual support facilitates recall. Journal of Cognition and Development, 14(2), 343-358.
Halpin, J. A., Puff, C. R., Mason, H. F., & Marston, S. P. (1984). Self-reference encoding and incidental recall by children. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 22(2), 87-89.
Hay, D. F. (2006). Yours and mine:Toddlers' talk about possessions with familiar peers. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24(1), 39-52.
Holm, S. (1979). A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure. Scandinavian Journal of Statistics, 6(2), 65-70.
Johnson, M. K., Hashtroudi, S., & Lindsay, D. S. (1993). Source monitoring. Psychological Bulletin, 114(1), 3-28.
Lakens, D. (2013). Calculating and reporting effect sizes to facilitate cumulative science:A practical primer for t-tests and ANOVAs. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 863-874.
Pullyblank, J., Bisanz, J., Scott, C., & Champion, M. A. (1985). Developmental invariance in the effects of functional self-knowledge on memory. Child Development, 56(6), 1447-1454.
Ross, H. S. (1996). Negotiating principles of entitlement in sibling property disputes. Developmental Psychology, 32(1), 90-101.
Ross, J., Anderson, J. R., & Campbell, R. N. (2011). I remember me:Mnemonic self-reference effects in preschool children.Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 76(3), 1-16.
Rossion, B., & Pourtois, G. (2004). Revisiting Snodgrass and Vanderwart's object pictorial set:The role of surface detail in basic-level object recognition. Perception, 33(2), 217-236.
Sui, J., & Humphreys, G. W. (2015). The integrative self:How self-reference integrates perception and memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(12), 719-728.
Sui, J., & Humphreys, G. W. (2017). Aging enhances cognitive biases to friends but not the self. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24(6), 1-10.
Sui, J., & Zhu, Y. (2005). Five-year-olds can show the self-reference advantage. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 29(5), 382-387.
Symons, C. S., & Johnson, B. T. (1997). The self-reference effect in memory:A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 121(3), 371-394.
Turk, D. J., Gillespie-Smith, K., Krigolson, O. E., Havard, C., Conway, M. A., & Cunningham, S. J. (2015). Selfish learning:The impact of self-referential encoding on children's literacy attainment. Learning and Instruction, 40, 54-60.
周爱保, 刘沛汝, 史战, 张鹏英, 吴慧芬, 李琼. (2010). 四岁儿童的自我参照效应研究. 心理发展与教育, (3), 239-244.
朱滢, 张力. (2001). 自我记忆效应的实验研究. 中国科学(C辑:生命科学), 31(06), 537-543.
[1] 储月, 刘希平, 徐慧, 唐卫海. 儿童社会分享型提取诱发遗忘的发展特点[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(2): 153-159.
[2] 刘艳春, 邓玉婷, 张曦. 智力障碍儿童对不同对象的分享行为:心理理论的作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(2): 160-168.
[3] 梁丹丹, 闫晓民, 葛志林. 4~8岁汉语高功能自闭症儿童基于语言线索的情绪识别能力发展研究[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(2): 169-175.
[4] 史梦梦, 任桂琴, 孙军红, 张鑫星. 词汇类型和阅读水平对小学一年级儿童阅读理解监控的影响[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(2): 207-214.
[5] 冷欣怡, 苏萌萌, 李文玲, 杨秀杰, 邢爱玲, 张湘琳, 舒华. 家庭环境与农村儿童早期语言发展的关系[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(1): 8-18.
[6] 赵京伟, 陈晓旭, 任立文, 耿喆, 徐夫真. 父母心理控制与小学儿童焦虑:一个有调节的中介模型[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(1): 93-102.
[7] 朱娜平, 刘雁伶, 熊红梅, 赵攀. “公无远近”有早晚:不同资源分配情境中儿童公平规范执行行为发展特点[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(6): 772-780.
[8] 颜志强, 周可, 曾晓, 徐惠, 朱晓倩, 张娟. 学前期儿童执行功能与攻击性行为的关系:认知共情的中介作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(6): 788-797.
[9] 任立文, 马原驰, 张清瑶, 张玲玲, 徐夫真. 父母心理控制与儿童学校适应的关系:基于交叉滞后的分析[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(5): 635-644.
[10] 李甜甜, 董会芹. 父母冲突知觉与儿童焦虑情绪:正负性信息注意偏向的中介作用及性别的调节作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(4): 488-496.
[11] 银小兰, 周路军, 朱翠英. 学校环境对农村留守儿童亲社会行为的影响:心理资本与生活满意度的中介作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(4): 497-504.
[12] 赵纤, 王志航, 王东方, 袁言云, 尹霞云, 黎志华. 贫困家庭儿童在青少年早期的亲社会行为发展轨迹:性别及父母教养方式异质性的影响[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(3): 323-332.
[13] 张玉平, 董琼, 宋爽, 舒华. 小学低年级儿童的阅读发展轨迹:早期语言认知技能的预测作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(2): 210-218.
[14] 李君, 王悦, 陈夏妮, 李莹. 二语学习对汉语学龄儿童认知控制与词汇通达的作用——年龄和二语熟练程度的影响[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(2): 219-227.
[15] 鲁明辉, 王融, 张丽敏. 父母心理弹性与自闭症谱系障碍儿童情绪行为问题的关系:一个有调节的中介模型[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(2): 247-254.
Viewed
Full text


Abstract

Cited

  Shared   
  Discussed   
No Suggested Reading articles found!