心理发展与教育 ›› 2024, Vol. 40 ›› Issue (3): 367-374.doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2024.03.07

• 教与学心理 • 上一篇    下一篇

象似性手势和指示性手势对自闭症儿童动作相关词汇学习的影响

王娟, 刘天碧, 范小月   

  1. 江苏师范大学教育科学学院, 徐州 221116
  • 发布日期:2024-05-15
  • 通讯作者: 王娟 E-mail:juanzi0809@126.com
  • 基金资助:
    教育部人文社会科学研究青年基金项目“母亲语言支架对自闭症儿童叙事发展的影响及干预研究”(20YJCZH152);江苏师范大学研究生科研与实践创新计划项目“象似性手势促进自闭症儿童汉语动作动词泛化的干预研究”(2022XKT0491);江苏省高校“青蓝工程”优秀青年骨干教师人才项目。

The Effects of Iconic Gestures and Deictic Gestures on Action-related Vocabulary Learning in Children with Autism

WANG Juan, LIU Tianbi, FAN Xiaoyue   

  1. School of Education Science, Jiangsu Normal University, Xuzhou 221116
  • Published:2024-05-15

摘要: 本研究考察了自闭症儿童在象似性手势、指示性手势和无手势条件下的动作相关词汇的学习情况。选取三组语言水平匹配的高功能自闭症儿童,分别在象似性手势、指示性手势和无手势条件下进行动作相关词汇的学习。在第一次学习结束(T1)和最后一次学习结束2~3天后(T2)分别进行词汇理解和词汇命名测试。结果发现:(1)与其他手势类型相比,象似性手势对自闭症儿童的词汇理解有明显的促进作用;(2)无论哪个组别,学习次数的增加均有助于词汇学习成绩的提高;(3)对词汇命名,所有组别在短期学习条件下的表现都较差,但随着学习次数的增加,象似性手势对词汇命名的促进作用逐渐凸显。研究表明,象似性手势对自闭症儿童动作相关词汇的学习有一定的促进作用;命名任务下象似性手势的促进作用受到学习次数的调节,当学习次数足够多时,象似性手势的优势才得以体现;指示性手势对自闭症儿童动作相关词汇的学习并未表现出促进作用。

关键词: 象似性手势, 指示性手势, 自闭症儿童, 词汇理解, 词汇命名

Abstract: The present study aimed to investigate action-related vocabulary learning in children with autism under the conditions of iconic gesture, deictic gesture, and non-gesture. Three groups of language-level matched children were selected to learn eight action-related vocabularies under the conditions of iconic gesture, deictic gesture, and non-gesture. All children were trained through pictures, situational stories, and videos. The vocabulary comprehension test and vocabulary naming test were completed at the end of the first learning session (T1) and the last learning session (T2). The results showed that: (1) Iconic gestures significantly benefitted vocabulary comprehension in children with autism beyond deictic gesture and non-gesture; (2) The increase in the number of learning sessions contributed to the improvement of vocabulary learning performance, regardless of the group; (3) All groups’ naming scores were poor in the short-term learning condition, but as the number of learning sessions increased, the facilitative effect of iconic gesture on naming test became more prominent. The study has shown that iconic gestures can promote action-related vocabulary comprehension in children with autism. For the naming test, the promoting effect was regulated by the number of learning. The iconic gestures may successfully enhance vocabulary naming in children with autism by increasing the training intensity. In addition, deictic gestures do not significantly promote the action-related vocabulary learning of children with autism.

Key words: iconic gesture, deictic gesture, children with autism, vocabulary comprehension, vocabulary naming

中图分类号: 

  • G442
American Psychiatric Association. (2013).Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5). New York: American Psychiatric Pub.
Botting, N., Riches, N., Gaynor, M., & Morgan, G. (2010). Gesture production and comprehension in children with specific language impairment. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 28(1), 51-69.
Boucher, J., & Warrington, E. K. (1976). Memory deficits in early infantile autism: Some similarities to the amnesic syndrome.British Journal of Psychology, 67(1), 73-87.
Bowler, D. M., Matthews, N. J., & Gardiner, J. M. (1997). Asperger's syndrome and memory: Similarity to autism but not amnesia.Neuropsychologia, 35(1), 65-70.
Capone, N. C., & McGregor, K. K. (2005). The effect of semantic representation on toddlers’ word retrieval.Journal of Speech, Language & Hearing Research, 48(6), 1468-1480.
Charman, T. (2003). Why is joint attention a pivotal skill in autism? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 358(1430), 315-324.
Dimitrova, N., Özçalışkan, S., & Adamson, L. (2017). Do verbal children with autism comprehend gesture as readily as typically developing children?Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 47(10), 3267-3280.
Feyereisen, P. (2006). Further investigation on the mnemonic effect of gestures: Their meaning matters.European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 18(2), 185-205.
Hermelin, B., & O’Connor, N. (1967). Remembering of words by psychotic and subnormal children.British Journal of Psychology, 58(3-4), 213-218.
Holle, H. (2007).The comprehension of co-speech iconic gestures: behavioral, electrophysiological and neuroimaging studies (Unpublished doctorial dissertation). University of Leipzig.
Hostetter, A. B. (2011). When do gestures communicate? A meta-analysis.Psychological Bulletin, 137(2), 297-315.
Iverson, J. M., Capirci, O., & Caselli, M. C. (1994). From communication to language in two modalities.Cognitive Development, 9(1), 23-43.
Lavelli, M., Barachetti, C., & Florit, E. (2015). Gesture and speech during shared book reading with preschoolers with specific language impairment.Journal of Child Language, 42(6), 1191-1218.
Levelt, W. J., M., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production.Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(1), 1-75.
Macoun, A., & Sweller, N. (2016).Listening and watching: The effects of observing gesture on preschoolers’ narrative comprehension. Cognitive Development, 40, 68-81.
Matthews, D., Behne, T., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Origins of the human pointing gesture: A training study.Developmental Science, 15(6), 817-829.
McNeill, D. (1985). So you think gestures are nonverbal? Psychological Review, 92(3), 350-371.
McNeill, D. (1994). Hand and mind: What gestures reveal about thought. Language and Speech, 37(2), 203-209.
Meara, P. (2002). The rediscovery of vocabulary.Second Language Research, 18(4), 393-407.
Mottron, L., Morasse, K., & Belleville, S. (2001). A study of memory functioning in individualswith autism. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 42(2), 253-260.
Mumford, K. H., & Kita, S. (2016). At 10-12 months, pointing gesture handedness predicts the size of receptive vocabularies.Infancy, 21(6), 751-765.
Mundy, P., & Crowson, M. (1997). Joint attention and early social communication: Implications for research on intervention with autism.Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27(6), 653-676.
Novack, M. A., Goldin-Meadow, S., & Woodward, A. L. (2015). Learning from gesture: How early does it happen? Cognition, 142, 138-147.
Özçalışkan, S., Adamson, L. B., & Dimitrova, N. (2016). Early deictic but not other gestures predict later vocabulary in both typical development and autism. Autism, 20(6), 754-763.
Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (2001). Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action.Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2(9), 661-670.
Rowe, M. L., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2009a). Differences in early gesture explain SES disparities in child vocabulary size at school entry.Science, 323(5916), 951-953.
Rowe, M. L., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2009b). Early gesture selectively predicts later language learning.Developmental Science, 12(1), 182-187.
Sekine, K., Sowden, H., & Kita, S. (2015). The development of the ability to semantically integrate information in speech and iconic gesture in comprehension.Cognitive Science, 39(8), 1855-1880.
Silverman, L. B., Bennetto, L., Campana, E., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2010). Speech-and-gesture integration in high functioning autism.Cognition, 115(3), 380-393.
Tellier, M. (2008). The effect of gestures on second language memorisation by young children.Gesture, 8(2), 219-235.
Toichi, M., & Kamio, Y. (2002). Long-term memory and levels-of-processing in autism.Neuropsychologia, 40(7), 964-969.
Vogt, S., & Kauschke, C. (2017a). With some help from others’ hands: Iconic gesture helps semantic learning in children with specific language impairment.Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 60(11), 3213-3225.
Vogt, S., & Kauschke, C. (2017b). Observing iconic gestures enhances word learning in typically developing children and children with specific language impairment.Journal of Child Language, 44(6), 1458-1484.
Weismer, S. E., & Hesketh, L. J. (1993). The influence of prosodic and gestural cues on novel word acquisition by children with specific language impairment.Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 36(5), 1013-1025.
Wong, C., & Kasari, C. (2012). Play and joint attention of children with autism in the preschool special education classroom.Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(10), 2152-2161.
Yu, C., & Smith, L. B. (2012). Embodied attention and word learning by toddlers.Cognition, 125(2), 244-262.
曹漱芹. (2009). 汉语自闭症儿童视觉性语义理解机制的探索及应用研究 (博士学位论文). 华东师范大学, 上海.
陈璐, 张婷, 李泉, 冯廷勇. (2015). 孤独症儿童共同注意的神经基础及早期干预. 心理科学进展, 23(7), 1205-1215.
何旭良, 杨峰. (2018). 自闭症儿童的语言研究现状及构想. 医学与哲学, 39(8), 25-28.
王辉, 李广政. (2021). 儿童手势及其与学习的关系. 心理科学进展, 29(9), 1617-1627.
王娟, 张攀. (2021). 手势对3~4岁儿童汉语词汇学习的影响. 幼儿教育, (12), 14-18.
伍珍, 郭睿. (2017). 婴儿指示性手势与其语言学习的关系. 心理科学进展, 25(10), 1705-1712.
徐云, 张潘. (2020). 孤独症儿童共同注意研究现状与进展. 中国临床心理学杂志, 28(5), 924-927+931.
荀恩东, 饶高琦, 肖晓悦, 臧娇娇. (2016). 大数据背景下BCC语料库的研制. 语料库语言学, 3(1), 93-109+118.
杨慧, 孙玉梅. (2018). 自闭症儿童语言干预中的手势语辅助研究综述. 现代特殊教育, (8), 66-74.
张攀, 王娟. (2021). 象似性手势在儿童早期词汇学习中的作用. 学前教育研究, (4), 48-57.
[1] 梁丹丹, 闫晓民, 葛志林. 4~8岁汉语高功能自闭症儿童基于语言线索的情绪识别能力发展研究[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(2): 169-175.
[2] 关文军, 胡梦娟, 刘晨. 自闭症儿童父母歧视知觉对群际关系的影响:一个有调节的中介作用模型[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2021, 37(6): 854-863.
[3] 王菡, 俞稼钰, 梁丹丹. 汉语高功能自闭症儿童代词加工:利用提及顺序和重读线索的表现[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2021, 37(4): 481-488.
[4] 马玉, 张学民, 张盈利, 魏柳青. 自闭症儿童视觉动态信息的注意加工特点——来自多目标追踪任务的证据[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2013, 29(6): 571-577.
[5] 苏周简开. 香港学前儿童粤语词汇理解能力的发展[J]. 心理发展与教育, 1994, 10(2): 11-14.
Viewed
Full text


Abstract

Cited

  Shared   
  Discussed   
No Suggested Reading articles found!