心理发展与教育 ›› 2021, Vol. 37 ›› Issue (3): 353-362.doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2021.03.06

• 认知与社会性发展 • 上一篇    下一篇

认知抑制对科学创造力和艺术创造力的影响

姚海娟1, 仝娜1, 张珊珊2, 巩彦斌3   

  1. 1. 天津商业大学法学院, 天津 300134;
    2. 天津职业技术师范大学职业教育学院, 天津 300222;
    3. 太原理工大学国际教育交流学院, 太原 030024
  • 发布日期:2021-05-18
  • 通讯作者: 姚海娟 E-mail:yhjrenfei@163.com
  • 基金资助:
    天津市教育科学“十三五”规划课题“高等教育创新人才培养研究—非智力因素视角”(HE3018)。

The Effects of Cognitive Inhibition on Scientific Creativity and Artistic Creativity

YAO Haijuan1, TONG Na1, ZHANG Shanshan2, GONG Yanbin3   

  1. 1. School of Law, Tianjin University of Commerce, Tianjin 300134;
    2. School of Vocational Education, Tianjin University of Technology and Education, Tianjin 300222;
    3. College of International Education Exchange, Taiyuan University of Technology, Taiyuan 030024
  • Published:2021-05-18

摘要: 研究通过2个实验,考察不同高低抑制水平对科学创造力和艺术创造力的影响。通过安排不同难度的Simon任务(不一致试次占70%或10%)来消耗被试的认知资源,达到操纵被试的抑制水平,以侧抑制任务的前后测来评估被试抑制水平的变化情况,实验1和实验2分别以科学创造力测验和粘贴画任务测量被试的科学创造力和艺术创造力。结果发现,在科学创造力测验上,高抑制水平组被试的流畅性和灵活性得分显著高于低抑制水平组;在粘贴画任务中,低抑制水平组被试的创造性、可爱程度、综合印象得分以及总分均显著高于高抑制水平组。表明高认知抑制水平有利于科学创造力,低认知抑制水平有利于艺术创造力,认知抑制对科学创造力和艺术创造力的影响存在分离效应。

关键词: 认知抑制, 科学创造力, 艺术创造力

Abstract: Two experiments were conducted to explore the effects of cognitive inhibition on scientific creativity and artistic creativity. In this study, participants were arranged to complete Simon tasks with different difficulty levels (the proportion of inconsistent trials was 70% or 10%) to consume their resources for manipulating the inhibition level of participants, and Flanker task was conducted to evaluate the change of inhibition ability. Then, scientific creativity test was administered as the measurement of scientific creativity in experiment 1, while the collage task was administered as the measurement of artistic creativity in experiment 2. The results showed that:(1) in the scientific creativity test, the fluency and flexibility scores of high inhibition level group were significantly higher than those of the low inhibition level group; (2) in the collage task, the scores of creativity, cuteness, comprehensive impression and the total score of the low inhibition level group were significantly higher than those of the high inhibition level group. It was concluded that the cognitive inhibition of high level promoted scientific creativity, while the low level of cognitive inhibition promoted artistic creativity, cognitive inhibition had an effect on the separation of scientific creativity and artistic creativity.

Key words: scientific creativity, artistic creativity, cognitive inhibition

中图分类号: 

  • B844
Agnoli, S.,Corazza, G. E., & Runco, M. A. (2016). Estimating creativity with a multiple-measurement approach within scientific and artistic domains. Creativity Research Journal, 28(2), 171-176.
Amabile, T. M. (1982). Social psychology of creativity:A consensual assessment technique. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43(5), 997-1013.
Batey, M., & Furnham, A. (2008). The relationship between measures of creativity and schizotypy. Personality and Individual Differences, 45(8), 816-821.
Beaty, R. E., & Silvia, P. J. (2012). Why do ideas get more creative across time? An executive interpretation of the serial order effect in divergent thinking tasks.Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 6(4), 309-319.
Beaty, R. E., Silvia, P. J., Nusbaum, E. C., Jauk, E., & Benedek, M. (2014). The role of associative and executive processes in creative cognition.Memory & Cognition, 42(7), 1186-1197.
Benedek, M., Franz, F., Heene, M., & Neubauer, A. C. (2012). Differential effects of cognitive inhibition and intelligence on creativity. Personality and Individual Differences, 53(4), 480-485.
Benedek, M., Jauk, E., Fink, A., Koschutnig, K., Reishofer, G., Ebner, F., & Neubauer, A. C. (2014). To create or to recall? Neural mechanisms underlying the generation of creative new ideas.NeuroImage, 88, 125-133.
Benedek, M., Jauk, E., Sommer, M., Arendasy, M., & Neubauer, A. C. (2014). Intelligence, creativity, and cognitive control:The common anddifferential involvement of executive functions in intelligence and creativity. Intelligence, 46, 73-83.
Boccia, M., Piccardi, L., Palermo, L., Nori, R., & Palmiero, M. (2015). Where do bright ideas occur in the brain? Meta-analytic evidence from neuroimaging studies of domain-specific creativity.Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1195.
Carson, S., Peterson, J., & Higgins, D. M. (2003). Decreased latent inhibition is associated with increased creative achievement in high-functioning individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(3), 499-506.
Chamberlain, R., Swinnen, L., Heeren, S., & Wagemans, J. (2017). Perceptual flexibility is coupled with reduced executive inhibition in students of the visual arts.British Journal of Psychology, 109(2), 244-258.
Chamberlain, R., & Wagemans, J. (2015). Visual arts training is linked to flexible attention to local and global levels of visual stimuli.Acta Psychologica, 161, 185-197.
Cheng, L. F., Hu, W. P., Jia, X. J., & Runco, M. A. (2016). The different role of cognitive inhibition in early versus late creative problem finding.Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 10(1), 32-41.
Chirilaa,C., & Feldman, A. (2012). Study of latent inhibition at high-level creative personality:The link between creativity and psychopathology, Procedia-Social and Behavioral Sciences, 33, 353-357.
Edl, S., Benedek, M., Papousek, I., Weiss, E. M., & Fink, A. (2014). Creativity and the Stroop interference effect. Personality and Individual Differences, 69, 38-42.
Eriksen, B., & Eriksen, C. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task.Perception and Psychophysics, 16(1), 143-149.
Eysenck, H. J. (1995).Problems in the behavioural sciences, 12 Genius:The natural history of creativity. New York, NY, US:Cambridge University Press.
Feist, G. J. (1998). A meta-analysis of personality in scientific and artistic creativity. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2(4), 290-309.
Fink, A., Slamar-Halbedl, M., Unterrainer, H. F., & Weiss, E. (2012). Creativity:Genius, madness, or a combination of both? Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 6(1), 11-18.
Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2004). The relations among inhibition and interference control functions:A latent-variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology:General, 133(1), 101-135.
Gilhooly, K. J., Fioratou, E., Anthony, S. H., & Wynn, V. (2007). Divergent thinking:Strategies and executive involvement in generating novel uses for familiar objects.British Journal of Psychology, 98(4), 611-625.
Gonen-Yaacovi, G., de Souza, L. C., Levy, R., Urbanski, M., Josse, G., & Volle, E. (2013). Rostral and caudal prefrontal contribution to creativity:A meta-analysis of functional imaging data.Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 465.
Groborz, M., & Necka, E. (2003). Creativity and cognitive control:explorations of generation and evaluation skills.Creativity Research Journal, 15(2-3), 183-197.
Hagger, M. S., Wood, C., Stiff, C., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2010). Ego depletion and the strength model of self-control:A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 136(4), 495-525.
Hu, W. P., & Adey, P. (2002). A scientific creativity test for secondary school student.International Journal of Science Education, 24(4), 384-403.
Katrin, L., & Beat, M. (2016). Disentangling the impact of artistic creativity on creative thinking, working memory, attention, and intelligence:Evidence for domain-specific relationships with a new self-report questionnaire. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1089.
Kaufman, J. C. (2012). Counting the muse:Development of the Kaufman Domains of Creativity Scale (K-DOCS).Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 6(4), 298-308.
Martindale, C. (1999).Biological basis of creativity. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of creativity (pp. 137-152). New York:Cambridge University Press.
Martindale, C. (2007). Creativity, primordial cognition, and personality.Personality and Individual Differences, 43(7), 1777-1785.
Moreno, S., Wodniecka, Z., Tays, W., Alain, C., & Bialystok, E. (2014). Inhibitory Control in Bilinguals and Musicians:Event Related Potential (ERP) Evidence for Experience-Specific Effects.PLOS ONE, 9(4), e94169.https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0094169
Niu, W., & Sternberg, R. J. (2001). Cultural influences on artistic creativity and its evaluation.International Journal of Psychology, 36(4), 225-241.
Pérez-Fabello, María José., Campos, A., & Campos-Juanatey, D. (2016). Is object imagery central to artistic performance? Thinking Skills and Creativity, 21, 67-74.
Persson, J., Welsh, K. M., Jonides, J., & Reuter-Lorenz, P. A. (2007). Cognitive fatigue of executive processes:Interaction between interference resolution tasks.Neuropsychologia, 45(7), 1571-1579.
Peterson, J. B., Smith, K. W., & Carson, S. H. (2002). Openness and extraversion are associated with reduced latent inhibition:Replication and commentary. Personality and Individual Differences, 33(7), 1137-1147.
Radel, R., Davranche, K., Fournier, M., & Dietrich, A. (2015). The role of (dis) inhibition in creativity:Decreased inhibition improves idea generation. Cognition, 134, 110-120.
Shi, B. G., Cao, X. Q., Chen, Q. L., Zhuang, K. X., & Qiu, J. (2017). Different brain structures associated with artistic and scientific creativity:A voxel-based morphometry study. Scientific Reports, 7, 42911.
Simon, J. R. (1990). The effect of an irrelevant directional cue on human information processing. In R. W. Proctor & T. G. Reeve (Eds.),Stimulus-response compatibility:An integrated perspective (pp. 174-176). Amsterdam:North-Holland. 31-88.
Sternberg, R. J., & Lubart, T. I. (1991). An investment theory of creativity and its development. Human Development, 34(1), 1-31.
Storm, B. C., & Patel, T. N. (2014). Forgetting as a consequence and enabler of creative thinking.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40(6), 1594-1609.
Van Den Wildenber, W. P.M., Wyllie, S. A., Forstmann, B. U., Burle, B., Hasbroucq, T., & Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2010). To head or heed? Beyond the surface of selective action inhibition:A review. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4, 222.
White, H. A., & Shah, P. (2006). Uninhibited imaginations:Creativity in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Personality and Individual Differences, 40(6), 1121-1131.
Xue, Y. K., Gu, C. H, Wu, J. J., Dai, D. Y., Mu, X. L, & Zhou, Z. K. (2018). The effects of extrinsic motivation on scientific and artistic creativity among middle school students. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 52, 1-14.
Yi, X. F, Plucker, J. A., & Guo, J. J. (2015). Modeling influences on divergent thinking and artistic creativity. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 16, 62-68.
Zabelina, D. L., Robinson, M. D., Council, J. R., & Bresin, K. (2012). Patterning and nonpatterning in creative cognition:Insights from performance in a random number generation task. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 6(2), 137-145.
Zhang, L. J., Qiao, L., Chen, Q. L., Yang, W. J., Xu, M. S., Yao, X. N., … Yang, D. (2016). Gray matter volume of the lingual gyrus mediates the relationship between inhibition function and divergent thinking. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1532.
Zeki, S. (2001). Artistic creativity and the brain. Science, 293(5527), 51-52.
白学军, 姚海娟. (2018). 高低创造性思维水平者的认知抑制能力:行为和生理的证据. 心理学报, 50(11), 1197-1211.
白学军, 巩彦斌, 胡卫平, 韩琴, 姚海娟. (2014). 不同科学创造力个体干扰抑制机制的比较. 心理与行为研究, 12(2), 151-155.
程丽芳, 胡卫平, 贾小娟. (2015). 认知抑制对艺术创造力的影响:认知风格的调节作用. 心理发展与教育, 31(3), 287-295.
程丽芳. (2015). 科学创造力和艺术创造力:认知控制的融合与分离效应(硕士学位论文). 陕西师范大学, 西安.
胡卫平, 程丽芳, 贾小娟, 韩蒙, 陈英和. (2015). 认知抑制对创造性科学问题提出的影响:认知风格的中介作用. 心理与行为研究, 13(6), 721-728.
胡卫平,俞国良.(2002).青少年的科学创造力研究.教育研究,(1),44-48.
林崇德. (1999). 培养和造就高素质的创造性人才. 北京师范大学学报(社会科学版), (1), 5-13.
沈汪兵, 刘昌, 王永娟. (2010). 艺术创造力的脑神经生理基础. 心理科学进展, 18(10), 1520-1528.
姚海娟, 白学军. (2014). 创造性思维与认知抑制的关系. 心理科学, 37(2), 316-321.
衣新发, 胡卫平. (2013). 科学创造力和艺术创造力:启动效应及领域影响. 心理科学进展, 21(1), 22-30.
衣新发, 林崇德, 蔡曙山, 黄四林, 陈桄, 罗良, 唐敏. (2011). 留学经验与艺术创造力. 心理科学, 34(1), 190-195.
张克, 杜秀敏, 仝宇光. (2017). 高低创造性思维水平者定向遗忘效应的差异研究. 心理科学, 40(3), 514-519.
[1] 陈琼, 赵俊峰, 谷璜, 邢小莉, 李晓铭. 听障儿童认知抑制控制的脑电特点[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2020, 36(3): 275-282.
[2] 官冬晓, 艾继如, 黄碧娟, 崔爽, 司继伟. 数学焦虑影响儿童的数量表征:认知抑制的调节作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2020, 36(1): 10-18.
[3] 程丽芳, 胡卫平, 贾小娟. 认知抑制对艺术创造力的影响:认知风格的调节作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2015, 31(3): 287-295.
[4] 胡卫平, 韩葵葵. 青少年科学创造力的理论研究与实践探索[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2015, 31(1): 44-50.
[5] 黄红清, 张卫. 指向性遗忘中认知抑制能力的发展[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2003, 19(1): 44-48.
[6] 申继亮, 胡卫平, 林崇德. 青少年科学创造力测验的编制[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2002, 18(4): 76-81.
Viewed
Full text


Abstract

Cited

  Shared   
  Discussed   
No Suggested Reading articles found!