心理发展与教育 ›› 2020, Vol. 36 ›› Issue (5): 633-640.doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2020.05.15

• 理论探讨与进展 • 上一篇    

儿童基于心理本质论的种族认知及其影响因素

李占星1, 倪晓莉1, 牛更枫1, 朱莉琪2,3   

  1. 1. 西安交通大学人文社会科学学院社会心理学研究所, 西安 710049;
    2. 中国科学院行为科学重点实验室, 中国科学院心理研究所, 北京 100101;
    3. 中国科学院大学心理系, 北京 100049
  • 发布日期:2020-09-12
  • 通讯作者: 朱莉琪 E-mail:zhulq@psych.ac.cn
  • 基金资助:
    中国博士后科学基金(2018M633484);西安交通大学基本科研业务费(SK2019018)。

Children's Racial/Ethnic Essentialism and its Contributing Factors

LI Zhanxing1, NI Xiaoli1, NIU Gengfeng1, ZHU Liqi2,3   

  1. 1. Institute of Social Psychology, School of Humanities and Social Sciences, Xi'an Jiaotong University, Xi'an 710049;
    2. CAS Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology, Beijing 100101;
    3. Department of Psychology, University of Chinese Academy of sciences, Beijing 100049
  • Published:2020-09-12

摘要: 心理本质论将类别成员视为由共同的本质所决定,且这些本质对应着一些表面的不可改变的属性。其中,种族本质论就是心理本质论的一种。研究种族本质论的发展对于了解儿童的种族认知具有重要意义。种族本质论者认为,种族具有遗传不变性、有较强的归纳强度和标签具有客观性等特征。但是,基于不同特征的种族本质论的发展存在差异。类属语言、社会文化背景和群体地位,均可能影响儿童种族本质论的发展。这些影响因素提示,可通过减少类属语言的描述和促进民族融合等方式改变儿童的种族本质论。比较儿童对种族本质论的建构与自然世界的建构之间的差异,方便我们探索类别学习跨领域的一致性问题。如何确保种族本质论的发展是适宜的,这需要未来开展一些干预性的研究。

关键词: 儿童, 心理本质论, 遗传稳定性, 归纳强度, 类属语言

Abstract: Psychological essentialism regards category members as determined by the common essence, which corresponds to some apparently immutable properties. Among them, racial/ethnic essentialism is a kind of psychological essentialism, which is of great significance to understand children's cognition of race/ethnicity. Individuals who hold racial/ethnic essentialism regard race/ethnicity as heritably unchanged, having strong inductive potential, and label of race/ethnicity being objective. However, children's racial/ethnic essentialism basing on different features have different developmental trajectory. Use of generic language, social cultural contexts and the group status all will impact children's belief of racial/ethnic essentialism, which suggests we may weaken children's belief of racial/ethnic essentialism by decreasing usage of generic language and promoting national integration. To compare the developmental trajectory between children's racial/ethnic essentialism and natural essentialism may help us understand the cross-domain consistency in category learning for children. Some intervention studies may be needed to ensure the development of racial/ethnic essentialism is appropriate.

Key words: children, psychological essentialism, genetic stability, inductive potential, generic language

中图分类号: 

  • B844
Bangs, R., & Davis, L. E. (2015). Race and social problems:Restructuring inequality. Now York:Springer.
Birnbaum, D., Deeb, I., Segall, G., Ben-Eliyahu, A., & Diesendruck, G. (2010). The development of social essentialism:The case of Israeli children's inferences about Jews and Arabs. Child Development, 81(3), 757-777.
Chao, M. M., Chen, J., Roisman, G. I., & Hong, Y. (2010). Essentializing race:Implications for bicultural individuals' cognition and physiological reactivity. Psychological Science, 18(4), 341-348.
Chao, M. M., Hong, Y., & Chiu, C. (2013). Essentializing race:Its implications on racial categorization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104(4),619-634.
Dai, Q., McGregor, E., & Williams, J. (2015). Being Chinese or being Scottish:National identification and intergroup attitudes of British born Chinese children. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 12(3), 282-296.
De Caroli, M. E., Falanga, R., & Sagone, E. (2013). Ethnic awareness, self-identification, and attitudes toward ingroup and outgroup in Italian, Chinese and African pupils. Procedia-Social and Behavioral Sciences, 93(21), 444-448.
Deeb, I., Segall, G., Birnbaum, D., Beneliyahu, A.,& Diesendruck, G. (2011). Seeing isn't believing:The effect of intergroup exposure on children's essentialist beliefs about ethnic categories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101(6), 1139-1156.
Diesendruck, G., Birnbaum, D., Deeb, I., & Segall, G. (2013). Learning what is essential relative and absolute changes in children's beliefs about the heritability of ethnicity. Journal of Cognition and Development, 14(4), 546-560.
Diesendruck, G., & haLevi, H. (2006). The role of language, appearance, and culture in children's social category-based induction. Child Development, 77(3), 539-553.
Diesendruck, G., Goldfein-Elbaz, R., Rhodes, M., Gelman, S., & Neumark, N. (2013). Cross-cultural differences in children's beliefs about the objectivity of social categories. Child Development, 84(6), 1906-1917.
Diesendruck, G., & Menahem, R. (2015). Essentialism promotes children's inter-ethnic bias. Frontiers in Psychology, 6,1180.
Gaither, S. E., Schultz, J. R., Pauker, K., Sommers, S. R., Maddox, K. B., & Ambady, N. (2014). Essentialist thinking predicts decrements in children's memory for racially ambiguous faces. Developmental Psychology, 50(2), 482-488.
Gelman, S.A. (2003). The essential child:Origins of essentialism in everyday thought. New York:Oxford University Press.
Gelman, S. A. (2004). Psychological essentialism in children. Trends in Cognitive Science, 8(9), 404-409.
Gelman, S. A. (2009). Essentialist reasoning about the biological world. In A. Berthoz & Y. Christen (Eds). Neuro-biology of "Umwelt":How living beings perceive the world. Berlin:Springer.
Gelman,S. A., Goetz, P. J., Sarnecka, B. W., & Flukes, J. (2008). Generic language in parent-child conversations. Language Learning and Development, 4(1), 1-31.
Gelman, S. A., & Roberts, S. O. (2017). How language shapes the cultural inheritance of categories. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 114(30), 7900-7907.
Gelman, S. A., & Wellman, H, M. (1991). Insides and essences:Early understanding of the non-obvious. Cognition, 38, 213-244.
Haslam, N., Rothschild, L., & Ernst, D. (2000). Essentialist beliefs about social categories. British Journal of Social Psychology, 39(1),113-127.
Haslam, N., Rothschild, L., & Ernst, D. (2002). Are essentialist beliefs associated with prejudice? British Journal of Social Psychology, 41(1), 87-100.
Heyer, G. (1985). Generic descriptions, default reasoning, and typicality. Theoretical Linguistics, 12(1), 33-72.
Hirschfeld, L. A. (1995a). Do children have a theory of race? Cognition, 54(2), 209-252.
Hirschfeld, L. A. (1995b). The inheritability of identity:Children's understanding of the cultural biology of race. Child Development, 66(5),1418-1437.
Hirschfeld, L. A. (1996). Race in the making:Cognition, culture, and the child's construction of human kinds. Cambridge, MA:MIT Press
Jayaratne, T. E., Ybarra, O., Sheldon, J. P, Brown, T. N., Feldbaum, M., & Pfeffer, C. A. (2006). White Americans' genetic lay theories of race differences and sexual orientation:Their relationship with prejudice toward Blacks, and gay men and lesbians. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 9(1),77-94.
Kinzler, K. D., & Dautel, J. B. (2012). Children's essentialist reasoning about language and race. Developmental Science, 15(1), 131-138.
Kinzler, K. D., Shutts, K., & Correll, J. (2010). Priorities in social categories. European Journal of Social Psychology, 40(4), 581-592.
Levy, S. R, & Dweck, C. S. (1999). The impact of children's static versus dynamic conceptions of people on stereotype formation. Child Development, 70(5), 1163-1180.
Mahalingam, R. (2007). Essentialism, power, and the representation of social categories:A folk sociology perspective. Human Development, 50(6),300-319.
Mandalaywala, T. M., Amodio, D. M., & Rhodes, M. (2017). Essentialism promotes racial prejudice by increasing endorsement of social hierarchies. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 9(4), 461-469.
Mandalaywala, T. M., Ranger-Murdock, G., Amodio, D. M., & Rhodes, M. (2019). The nature and consequences of essentialist beliefs about race in early childhood. Child Development, in press.
Mandalaywala, T. M., & Rhodes, M. (2016). Racial essentialism is associated with prejudice towards blacks in 5-and 6-year-old white children. The Annual Meeting of The Cognitive Science Society, 1319-1324.
Nei, M., & Roychoudhury, A. K. (1982). Genetic relationship and evolution of human races. Evolutionary Biology, 14, 1-59.
Pauker, K., Ambady, N., & Apfelbaum, E. P. (2010). Race salience and essentialist thinking in racial stereotype development. Child Development, 81(6), 1799-1813.
Pauker, K., Carpinella, C., Meyers, C., Young, D. M., & Sanchez, D. T. (2018). The role of diversity exposure in Whites' reduction in race essentialism over time. Social Psychological and Personality Science, in press.
Pauker, K., Xu, Y., Williams, A., & Biddle, A. M. (2016). Race essentialism and social contextual differences in children's racial stereotyping. Child Development, 87(5), 1409-1422.
Pereira, M. E., Estramiana, J. L., & Gallo, I. S. (2010). Essentialism and the expression of social stereotypes:A comparative study of Spain, Brasil and England. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 13(2), 808-817.
Prentice, D. A., & Miller, D. T. (2007). Psychological essentialism of human categories. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(4), 202-206.
Quintana,S. M., Benjamin,J. Z., & Leverett, P. (2017). Essentialism and children's reasoning about race and ethnicity. In A. Rutland, D. Nesdale, & C. Brown. The Wiley Handbook of Group Processes in Children and Adolescents (pp. 292-313). New York:John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
Rhodes, M. (2013). How two intuitive theories shape the development of social categorization. Child Development Perspectives, 7(1), 12-16.
Rhodes, M. (2014). Children's explanations as a window into their intuitive theories of the social world. Cognitive Science, 38(8), 1687-1697.
Rhodes, M., & Gelman, S. A. (2009). A developmental examination of the conceptual structure of animal, artifact, and human social categories across two cultural contexts. Cognitive Psychology, 59(3), 244-274.
Rhodes, M., Leslie, S. J., Bianchi, L., & Chalik, L. (2018). The role of generic language in the early development of social categorization. Child Development, 89(1),148-155.
Rhodes, M., Leslie, S. J., & Tworek, C. M. (2012). Cultural transmission of social essentialism. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109(34), 13526-13531.
Rhodes, M., & Mandalaywala, T. M. (2017). The development and developmental consequences of social essentialism. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews Cognitive Science, 8(4), e1437.
Roberts, S. O., Ho, A. K, Rhodes, M., & Gelman, S. A. (2017). Making boundaries great again:Essentialism and support for boundary-enhancing initiatives. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 43(12), 1643-1658.
Roberts, S. O., & Gelman, S. A. (2016). Can white children grow up to be black? Children's reasoning about the stability of emotion and race. Developmental Psychology, 52(6), 887-893.
Roberts, S. O., & Gelman, S. A. (2017). Now you see race, now you don't:Verbal cues influence children's racial stability judgments. Cognitive Development, 43, 129-141.
Ryazanov, A. A., & Christenfeld, N. J. (2018). The strategic value of essentialism. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 12(1), e12370.
Segall, G., Birnbaum, D., Deeb, I., & Diesendruck, G. (2015). The intergenerational transmission of ethnic essentialism:How parents talk counts the most. Developmental Science, 18(4), 543-555.
Shutts K. (2015). Young children's preferences:Gender, race, and social status. Child Development Perspectives, 9(4), 262-266.
Siebers, H. (2017). "Race" versus "ethnicity"? Critical race essentialism and the exclusion and oppression of migrants in the Netherlands. Ethnic and Racial Studies, 40(3), 369-387.
Tadmor, C. T., Chao, M. M., Hong, Y., & Polzer, J. (2013). Not just for stereotyping anymore. Psychological Science, 24(1), 99-105.
Wellmen, H. M., & Gelman, S. A. (1992). Cognitive development:Foundational theories of core domains. Annual Review of Psychology, 43(13), 337-375.
Yzerbyt, V., Corneille, O., & Estrada, C. (2001). The interplay of subjective essentialism and entitativity in the formation of stereotypes. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5(2), 141-155.
高成海, 万明刚. (2013). 民族本质论对民族认同和刻板印象的影响. 心理学报, 45(2), 231-242.
宫玉宽. (2013). 民族认同与宗教对我国少数民族民族认同的影响. 西北民族大学学报(哲学社会科学版), (2), 18-24.
韦磐石, 张翔. (2013). 少数民族青少年国家认同和民族认同研究. 云南行政学院学报, (2), 40-43.
杨晓莉, 刘力, 赵显, 史佳鑫. (2014). 民族本质论对跨民族交往的影响——以中国内地的藏族大学生为例. 心理科学, 37(2), 394-399.
[1] 储月, 刘希平, 徐慧, 唐卫海. 儿童社会分享型提取诱发遗忘的发展特点[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(2): 153-159.
[2] 刘艳春, 邓玉婷, 张曦. 智力障碍儿童对不同对象的分享行为:心理理论的作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(2): 160-168.
[3] 梁丹丹, 闫晓民, 葛志林. 4~8岁汉语高功能自闭症儿童基于语言线索的情绪识别能力发展研究[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(2): 169-175.
[4] 史梦梦, 任桂琴, 孙军红, 张鑫星. 词汇类型和阅读水平对小学一年级儿童阅读理解监控的影响[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(2): 207-214.
[5] 冷欣怡, 苏萌萌, 李文玲, 杨秀杰, 邢爱玲, 张湘琳, 舒华. 家庭环境与农村儿童早期语言发展的关系[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(1): 8-18.
[6] 赵京伟, 陈晓旭, 任立文, 耿喆, 徐夫真. 父母心理控制与小学儿童焦虑:一个有调节的中介模型[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(1): 93-102.
[7] 朱娜平, 刘雁伶, 熊红梅, 赵攀. “公无远近”有早晚:不同资源分配情境中儿童公平规范执行行为发展特点[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(6): 772-780.
[8] 颜志强, 周可, 曾晓, 徐惠, 朱晓倩, 张娟. 学前期儿童执行功能与攻击性行为的关系:认知共情的中介作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(6): 788-797.
[9] 任立文, 马原驰, 张清瑶, 张玲玲, 徐夫真. 父母心理控制与儿童学校适应的关系:基于交叉滞后的分析[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(5): 635-644.
[10] 李甜甜, 董会芹. 父母冲突知觉与儿童焦虑情绪:正负性信息注意偏向的中介作用及性别的调节作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(4): 488-496.
[11] 银小兰, 周路军, 朱翠英. 学校环境对农村留守儿童亲社会行为的影响:心理资本与生活满意度的中介作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(4): 497-504.
[12] 赵纤, 王志航, 王东方, 袁言云, 尹霞云, 黎志华. 贫困家庭儿童在青少年早期的亲社会行为发展轨迹:性别及父母教养方式异质性的影响[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(3): 323-332.
[13] 张玉平, 董琼, 宋爽, 舒华. 小学低年级儿童的阅读发展轨迹:早期语言认知技能的预测作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(2): 210-218.
[14] 李君, 王悦, 陈夏妮, 李莹. 二语学习对汉语学龄儿童认知控制与词汇通达的作用——年龄和二语熟练程度的影响[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(2): 219-227.
[15] 鲁明辉, 王融, 张丽敏. 父母心理弹性与自闭症谱系障碍儿童情绪行为问题的关系:一个有调节的中介模型[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(2): 247-254.
Viewed
Full text


Abstract

Cited

  Shared   
  Discussed   
No Suggested Reading articles found!