心理发展与教育 ›› 2018, Vol. 34 ›› Issue (4): 443-452.doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2018.04.08

• 教与学心理 • 上一篇    下一篇

4~8年级学生分数数量表征的准确性及形式

高瑞彦1, 牛美心2, 杨涛2, 周新林3   

  1. 1. 北京师范大学中国基础教育质量监测协同创新中心, 北京 100875;
    2. 北京市大兴区地方税务局, 北京 102600;
    3. 北京师范大学认知神经科学与学习国家重点实验室, 北京 100875
  • 出版日期:2018-07-15 发布日期:2018-07-15
  • 通讯作者: 杨涛,E-mail:yangtao@bnu.edu.cn E-mail:yangtao@bnu.edu.cn
  • 基金资助:

    国家自然科学基金面上项目(31671151)。

The Accuracy and Form of Fraction Magnitude Representation for 4 to 8 Graders

GAO Ruiyan1, NIU Meixin2, YANG Tao2, ZHOU Xinlin3   

  1. 1. Collaborative Innovation Center of Assessment toward Basic Education Quality, Beijing Normal University, Beijing 100875;
    2. Beijing Daxing Local Taxation Bureau, Beijing 102600;
    3. National Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning, Beijing Normal University, Beijing 100875
  • Online:2018-07-15 Published:2018-07-15

摘要: 在分数学习中,分数数量表征是非常重要的方面。本研究利用数字线估计任务对四到八年级学生分数数量表征的情况进行探索。研究结果表明:随着年级升高,被试分数数量表征的准确性也随之提高;被试对单位分数和非单位分数表征的准确性存在显著差异,表现为对单位分数表征的准确性显著高于对非单位分数的表征,这种差异在低年级显著,随着年级的增长,差异逐渐消失;四到八年级学生在0~3数字线上,对分数数量的表征表现为线性形式而非对数形式,且这种线性表征形式是在六年级开始出现并随着年龄增长逐渐发展起来的。中西方儿童在分数数量表征的准确性和形式上有相似的发展路径,但是在表征准确性上中国儿童更高、线性形式出现年级上中国儿童可能更早。

关键词: 分数数量表征, 数字线估计, 准确性, 线性表征, 儿童

Abstract: Fraction is a useful tool for exploring the relationship between the two quantities in real life, and fraction magnitude representation plays an important role in fraction study. This study adopted number line task to investigate the patterns of fraction magnitude representation for fourth to eighth graders. The results demonstrated that:(1) The accuracy of fraction magnitude representation raised along with grade increasing; (2) The accuracy of unit fraction representation and non-unit fraction representation was different, the representation of unit fraction was significantly more accurate than non-unit fraction representation, furthermore, this difference was larger in junior grades (fourth to sixth graders) and could be ignorable in senior grades (seventh to eighth graders); (3) Students showed linear rather than logarithmic form in fraction magnitude representation on the 0~3 number line, the linear representation emerged in sixth graders and developed gradually with age increasing. Children in China and the West have similar development path for the accuracy and form of fraction magnitude representation, while Chinese children showed higher accuracy, and the linear representation emerged earlier.

Key words: fraction magnitude representation, number line estimation, accuracy, linear representation, children

中图分类号: 

  • G442

Bailey, D. H., Hoard, M. K., Nugent, L., & Geary, D. C. (2012). Competence with fractions predicts gains in mathematics achievement. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 113(3), 447-455.

Bailey, D. H., Zhou, X., Zhang, Y., Cui, J., Fuchs, L. S., Jordan, N. C., et al. (2015). Development of fraction concepts and procedures in u.s. and chinese children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 129, 68-83.

Berch, Daniel B., Foley, Elizabeth J., Hill, Rebecca J., & Ryan, Patricia McDonough. (1999). Extracting parity and magnitude from Arabic numerals:Developmental changes in number processing and mental representation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 74(4), 286-308.

Bonato, M., Fabbri, S., Umilta, C., & Zorzi, M. (2007). The mental representation of numerical fractions:real or integer?. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Human Perception and Performance, 33(6), 1410-1419.

Booth, J. L., & Newton, K. J. (2012). Fractions:Could they really be the gatekeeper's doorman? Contemporary Educational Psychology, 37(4), 247-253.

Booth, J. L., Newton, K. J., & Twissgarrity, L. K. (2014). The impact of fraction magnitude knowledge on algebra performance and learning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 118(1), 110-118.

Booth, J. L., & Siegler, R. S. (2006). Developmental and individual differences in pure numerical estimation. Developmental Psychology, 42(1), 189-201.

Dehaene, S. (1997). The number sense:How the mind creates mathematics. New York:Oxford University Press, 53-76.

Dehaene, S., & Mehler, J. (1992). Cross-linguistic regularities in the frequency of number words. Cognition, 12(1), 119-149.

Fazio, L. K., Bailey, D. H., Thompson, C. A., & Siegler, R. S. (2014). Relations of different types of numerical magnitude representations to each other and to mathematics achievement. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 123(123), 53-72.

Girelli, L., Lucangeli, D., & Butterworth, B. (2000). The development of automaticity in accessing number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 76(2), 104-122.

Hevia, M. D. D., & Spelke, E. S. (2009). Spontaneous mapping of number and space in adults and young children. Cognition, 110(2), 198-207.

Ischebeck, A., Schocke, M., & Delazer, M. (2009). The processing and representation of fractions within the brain:An fmri investigation. Neuroimage, 47(1), 403-413.

Jacob, S. N., & Nieder, A. (2009). Notation-independent representation of fractions in the human parietal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience the Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 29(14), 4652-4657.

Lemaire, P., & Lecacheur, M. (2002). Children's strategies in computational estimation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 82(4), 281-304.

Liu, C. H., Xin, Z. Q., Lin, C. D., & Thompson, C. A. (2013). Children's mental representation when comparing fractions with common numerators. Educational Psychology, 33(2), 175-191.

Liu, Y. (2017). Fraction magnitude understanding and its unique role in predicting general mathematics achievement at two early stages of fraction instruction. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 87(5), 1-18.

Mullis, I. V. S., Martin, M. O., Foy, P., Olson, J. F., Preuschoff, C., Erberber, E., et al. (2008). TIMSS 2007 international mathematics report:Findings from IEA's trends in international mathematics and science study at the fourth and eighth grades. Chestnut Hill, MA:TIMSS & PIRLS International Study Center, Boston College. 31-42.

Mullis, I. V. S., Martin, M. O., Foy, P., & Arora, A. (2012). TIMSS 2011 international results in mathematics. Chestnut Hill, MA:TIMSS & PIRLS International Study Center, Boston College. 36-50.

Opfer, J. E., & Devries, J. M. (2008). Representational change and magnitude estimation:Why young children can make more accurate salary comparisons than adults. Cognition, 108(3), 843-849.

Opfer, J. E., Thompson, C. A., & Devries, J. M. (2007). Why children make better estimates of fractional magnitude than adults. Xxix Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. 1361-1366.

Schneider, M., & Siegler, R. S. (2010). Representations of the magnitudes of fractions. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception & Performance, 36(5), 1227-1238.

Siegler, R. S. (1996). Emerging minds:The process of change in children's thinking. New York:Oxford University Press, 49-83.

Siegler, R. S., Duncan, G. J., Davis-Kean, P. E., Duckworth, K., Claessens, A., Engel, M., et al. (2012). Early predictors of high school mathematics achievement. Psychological Science, 23(7), 691-697.

Siegler, R. S., & Mu, Y. (2008). Chinese children excel on novel mathematics problems even before elementary school. Psychological Science, 19(8), 759-763.

Siegler, R. S., & Opfer, J. E. (2003). The development of numerical estimation:Evidence for multiple representations of numerical quantity. Psychological Science, 14(3), 237-243.

Siegler, R. S., Thompson, C. A., & Schneider, M. (2011). An integrated theory of whole number and fractions development. Cognitive Psychology, 62(4), 273-296.

Stapel, J. C., Hunnius, S., Bekkering, H., & Lindemann, O. (2015). The development of numerosity estimation:Evidence for a linear number representation early in life. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 27(4), 400-412.

Stevenson, H. W., Chen, C., & Uttal, D. H. (1990). Beliefs and achievement:A study of black, white, and hispanic children. Child Development, 61(2), 508-523.

Torbeyns, J., Schneider, M., Xin, Z., & Siegler, R. S. (2015). Bridging the gap:Fraction understanding is central to mathematics achievement in students from three different continents. Learning & Instruction, 37(3), 5-13.

Van Galen, M. S., & Reitsma, P. (2008). Developing access to number magnitude:A study of the SNARC effect in 7-to 9-year-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 101(2), 99-113.

Wang, Y. Q., & Siegler, R. S. (2013). Representations of and translation between common fractions and decimal fractions. Chinese Science Bulletin, 58(36), 4630-4640.

Zhou, Z., Peverly, S. T., & Lin, J. (2005). Understanding early mathematical competencies in american and chinese children. School Psychology International, 26(4), 413-427.

陈英和. (2015). 儿童数量表征与数概念的发展特点及机制. 心理发展与教育, 31(1), 21-28.

郭红力. (2010)小学高年级儿童的分数数量表征(硕士学位论文). 山东师范大学,济南.

卢淳, 郭红力, 司继伟, 孙燕. (2014). 不同数字线下儿童与成人分数估计的表征模式.心理发展与教育, 30(5), 449-456.

孙玉, 司继伟, 黄碧娟. (2016). 分数的数量表征.心理科学进展, 24(8), 1207-1216.

辛自强, 李丹. (2013). 小学生在非符号材料上的分数表征方式.心理科学,36(2), 364-371.

张帆, 赖颖慧, 陈英和. (2015). 儿童数字线表征的发展——心理长度的影响.心理发展与教育,31(2), 149-156.

张丽, 卢彩芳, 杨新荣. (2014). 3~6年级儿童整数数量表征与分数数量表征的关系.心理发展与教育,30(1), 1-8.

张丽, 辛自强, 王琦, 李红. (2012). 整数构成对分数加工的影响.心理发展与教育, 28(1), 31-38.

周广东, 莫雷, 温红博. (2009). 儿童数字估计的表征模式与发展. 心理发展与教育, 25(4), 21-29.
[1] 储月, 刘希平, 徐慧, 唐卫海. 儿童社会分享型提取诱发遗忘的发展特点[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(2): 153-159.
[2] 刘艳春, 邓玉婷, 张曦. 智力障碍儿童对不同对象的分享行为:心理理论的作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(2): 160-168.
[3] 梁丹丹, 闫晓民, 葛志林. 4~8岁汉语高功能自闭症儿童基于语言线索的情绪识别能力发展研究[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(2): 169-175.
[4] 史梦梦, 任桂琴, 孙军红, 张鑫星. 词汇类型和阅读水平对小学一年级儿童阅读理解监控的影响[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(2): 207-214.
[5] 冷欣怡, 苏萌萌, 李文玲, 杨秀杰, 邢爱玲, 张湘琳, 舒华. 家庭环境与农村儿童早期语言发展的关系[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(1): 8-18.
[6] 赵京伟, 陈晓旭, 任立文, 耿喆, 徐夫真. 父母心理控制与小学儿童焦虑:一个有调节的中介模型[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(1): 93-102.
[7] 朱娜平, 刘雁伶, 熊红梅, 赵攀. “公无远近”有早晚:不同资源分配情境中儿童公平规范执行行为发展特点[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(6): 772-780.
[8] 颜志强, 周可, 曾晓, 徐惠, 朱晓倩, 张娟. 学前期儿童执行功能与攻击性行为的关系:认知共情的中介作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(6): 788-797.
[9] 任立文, 马原驰, 张清瑶, 张玲玲, 徐夫真. 父母心理控制与儿童学校适应的关系:基于交叉滞后的分析[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(5): 635-644.
[10] 李甜甜, 董会芹. 父母冲突知觉与儿童焦虑情绪:正负性信息注意偏向的中介作用及性别的调节作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(4): 488-496.
[11] 银小兰, 周路军, 朱翠英. 学校环境对农村留守儿童亲社会行为的影响:心理资本与生活满意度的中介作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(4): 497-504.
[12] 赵纤, 王志航, 王东方, 袁言云, 尹霞云, 黎志华. 贫困家庭儿童在青少年早期的亲社会行为发展轨迹:性别及父母教养方式异质性的影响[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(3): 323-332.
[13] 张玉平, 董琼, 宋爽, 舒华. 小学低年级儿童的阅读发展轨迹:早期语言认知技能的预测作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(2): 210-218.
[14] 李君, 王悦, 陈夏妮, 李莹. 二语学习对汉语学龄儿童认知控制与词汇通达的作用——年龄和二语熟练程度的影响[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(2): 219-227.
[15] 鲁明辉, 王融, 张丽敏. 父母心理弹性与自闭症谱系障碍儿童情绪行为问题的关系:一个有调节的中介模型[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(2): 247-254.
Viewed
Full text


Abstract

Cited

  Shared   
  Discussed   
No Suggested Reading articles found!