Psychological Development and Education ›› 2014, Vol. 30 ›› Issue (3): 277-283.

Previous Articles     Next Articles

The Effects of Habitual Response and Agenda-Driven on Study Time Allocation:Evidence from Eye Movement

LI Wei-Jian1, XIE Rui-Bo1, CHEN Hai-De2, HUANG Jie1   

  1. 1. Institute of Psychology, Zhejiang Normal University, Jinhua 321004;
    2. Institute of Developmental Psychology, Beijing Normal University, Beijing 100875
  • Online:2014-05-15 Published:2014-05-15

Abstract: Ariel, Dunlosky and Bailey (2009) proposed Agenda-Based Regulation (ABR) framework. This explains the internal mechanisms of study time allocation from the perspective of information processing, that learners develop an agenda on how to allocate time to various study items and use this agenda when selecting items for study. When the limits of the central executive have been exceeded, habitual responses may gain control of study-time allocation.
This study aims to revealed the habitual response of the study time allocation. It further explored the relationship between the habitual response of the study time allocation and agenda-driven. Using Metcalfe's paradigm, two experiments independently were conducted in present research to examined twenty undergraduates's habitual response on study time allocation on condition of different time limit. Multi-factor experimental design was employed in this study. The independent variables were item difficulty, and words position, and the dependent variables were fixation duration and fixation points. On condition of no time limitation, the result revealed that habitual responses on item selection was found, but no effects on the self-paced study time. On condition of a time limit, there was no habitual response on study time allocation. Learners gave preference to easy items in all three words position. However, for easy items, there were also habitual responses. Which demonstrates learners' agendas can dominate item selection and self-paced study time, but can not completely eliminate the habitual responses to the item selection.

Key words: study time allocation, time limitation, agenda-driven, habitual response, eye tracking technique

CLC Number: 

  • G442
Ariel, R. (2012).Learning what to learn: the effects of task experience on strategy shifts in the allocation of study time. Unpublished doctorial dissertation, Kent State University.
Ariel, R., Al-Harthy, I. S., Was, C. A., & Dunlosky, J. (2011). Habitual reading biases in the allocation of study time.Psychonomic Bulletin &Rreview, 18(5), 1015-1021.
Ariel, R., & Dunlosky, J. (2013). When do learners shift from habitual to agenda-based processes when selecting items for study? Memory & Cognition, 41(3) 416-428.
Ariel, R., Dunlosky, J., & Bailey, H. (2009). Agenda-based regulation of study-time allocation: When agendas override item-based monitoring. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 138(3), 432-447.
Dunlosky, J., & Ariel, R. (2011a). Self-regulated learning and the allocation of study time. In B. H. Ross (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 54, pp. 103-140). San Diego:Academic.
Dunlosky, J., & Ariel, R. (2011b). The influence of agenda-based and habitual processes on item selection during study.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37(4), 899-912.
Dunlosky, J., & Hertzog, C. (1998). Training programs to improve learning in later adulthood: helping older adults educate themselves. In Hacker, D.J., Dunlosky, J., & Graesser, A.C. (Eds.), Metacognition in educational theory and practice. 1998. (pp. 249-275). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Dunlosky, J., & Thiede, K. W. (1998). What makes people study more? An evaluation of factors that affect self-paced study.Acta Psychologica, 98(1),37-56.
Metcalfe, J. (2002). Is study time allocated selectively to a region of proximal learning? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131, 349-363.
Metcalfe, J. (2009). Metacognitive judgments and control of study Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(6), 159-163.
Metcalfe, J., & Kornell, N. (2005). A region of proximal learning model of study time allocation. Journal of Memory and Language, 52(4), 463-477.
Metcalfe, J., & Shimamura, A. P. (Eds.). (1994). Metacognition: Knowing about knowing.Cambridge MA:The MIT Press.
Nelson, T. O., & Narens, L. (1990). Metamemory: A theoretical framework and new findings. In Bower, G. (Ed). The Psychology of learning and Motivation: Advances in Research and Theory. New York: Academic Press.
Nelson, T.O., & Narens, L. (1994). Why investigate metacognition? In Metcalfe, J., & Shimamura, A. P. (Eds.). Metacognition: Knowing about knowing(pp.1-25), Cambridge, MA:MIT Press.
Perfect, T. J., & Schwartz, B. L. (2002). Applied metacognition. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Price, J., Hertzog, C., & Dunlosky, J. (2010). Self-regulated learning in younger and older adults: does aging affect metacognitive control? Aging, neuropsychology, and cognition, 17(3), 329-359.
Soderstrom, N. C., & McCabe D. P. (2011). The interplay between value and relatedness as bases for metacognitive monitoring and control: evidence for agenda-based monitoring. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37(5):1236-1242.
Son, L. K., & Metcalfe, J. (2000). Metacognitive and control strategies in study-time allocation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26(1), 204-221.
Son, L. K., & Sethi, R. (2006). Metacognitive control and optimal learning. Cognitive Science, 30(4), 759-774.
Thiede, K. W., & Dunlosky, J. (1999). Toward a general model of self-regulated study: An analysis of selection of items for study and self-paced study time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25(4), 10-24.
Tiede, H. L., & Leboe, J. P. (2009). Metamemory judgments and the benefits of repeated study: Improving recall predictions through the activation of appropriate knowledge.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35(3), 822-828.
Waldron, S. M., Patrick, J., & Duggan, G. B. (2011). The influence of goal-state access cost on planning during problem solving. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(3), 485-503.
贾宁, 白学军, 臧传丽, 阎国利. (2008). 学习时间分配机制的眼动研究. 心理科学, 31 (1), 93-96.
李伟健. (2011). 学习困难儿童学习时间分配的特征. 博士学位论文, 北京师范大学.
[1] LI Yan, LIANG Lichan, ZHOU Xinran, BIAN Yufang. Parent-teacher Relationships and Primary Students’ School Attitude: The Mediating Role of Teacher-child Relationships and the Difference between Parents [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2024, 40(1): 74-82.
[2] YANG Baoyan, MA Xifeng, ZANG Hongyu, MAO Haiyan. The Influence of Family Economic Income and Father’s Education Level on Academic Engagement among Rural Junior Middle School Students:The Mechanisms of Family Obligation and Education-dependence Future Identity [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2024, 40(1): 83-92.
[3] WANG Haolan, CHEN Hongjun, WU Xinchun, ZHAO Ying, SUN Peng. The Effect of Reading Modes to Reading Comprehension in Chinese Children: A Mediated Moderation Model [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2023, 39(3): 369-378.
[4] CHENG Yangchun, HUANG Jin. Association between the Approximate Number System and Mathematical Competence: A Meta-analysis [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2023, 39(3): 379-390.
[5] YU Haoxue, LI Hui, WANG Fuxing. The Relationship between Belief in a Just World and Learning Burnout in Undergraduates: The Mediating Role of Coping Style and Boredom Proneness [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2023, 39(3): 391-401.
[6] CHEN Xu, ZHANG Dajun, CHEGN Gang, HU Tianqiang, LIU Guangzeng. The Effects of Teacher Support and Psychological Suzhi on Middle School Students' Academic Achievement [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2018, 34(6): 707-714.
[7] NIE Qian, ZHANG Dajun, TENG Zhaojun, LU Xingyue, GUO Cheng. How Students Perceived School Climate Effect on Subjective and Objective Academic Achievement: The Mediating Role of Psychological Suzhi [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2018, 34(6): 715-723.
[8] CHENG Gang, TANG Xinyi, NIU Juan, LI Jiajia, ZHANG Dajun. The Relationship between Middle School Students' Family Socioeconomic Status and Academic Achievement: Multiple Mediating Effects of Different Dimensions of Psychological Suzhi [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2018, 34(6): 700-706.
[9] CHEN Chaoyang, LIU Zhifang, SU Yongqiang, CHENG Yahua. The Prediction Effects for Skill and Less-skill Deaf Readers in Chinese Reading: Evidence from Eye Movement [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2018, 34(6): 692-699.
[10] ZOU Yuchen, DING Ying, ZHANG Xuran, LI Yanfang. The Effects of Feedback Type on Children's Learning Performance and the Gender Differences in These Effects [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2018, 34(5): 567-575.
[11] LI Yongzhan. The Impact of Parental Rearing Style on Learning Engagement among Senior High School Students: A Serial Mediation Effect Model [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2018, 34(5): 576-585.
[12] GAO Ruiyan, NIU Meixin, YANG Tao, ZHOU Xinlin. The Accuracy and Form of Fraction Magnitude Representation for 4 to 8 Graders [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2018, 34(4): 443-452.
[13] ZHANG Jie, HUANG Bijuan, SI Jiwei, GUAN Dongxiao. Relationship between Math Anxiety and Mathematical Achievement in Township Pupils: The Chain Mediating Roles of Mathematical Self-efficacy and Metacognition [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2018, 34(4): 453-460.
[14] WANG Mingzhong, WANG Mengran, WANG Jing. Interparental Conflict Compromises Adolescents' Academic Achievement: The Indirect Roles of Effortful Control and Classroom Engagement [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2018, 34(4): 434-442.
[15] CHI Liping, ZONG Zheng, XIN Ziqiang, CHEN Yinghe. The Relationship of Constructivist Pedagogy and Metathinking: The Mediating Role of Cognitive Holding Power [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2018, 34(2): 181-190.
Viewed
Full text


Abstract

Cited

  Shared   
  Discussed   
No Suggested Reading articles found!