Psychological Development and Education ›› 2021, Vol. 37 ›› Issue (4): 481-488.doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2021.04.04
Previous Articles Next Articles
WANG Han1, YU Jiayu2, LIANG Dandan2
CLC Number:
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA:American Psychiatric Publishing. Ariel, M. (1990). Accessing noun-phrase antecedents(pp.56-64). London:Routledge. Cascadilla Press. Arnold, J. E. (1998). Reference form and discourse patterns (unpublished doctoral dissertation). Stanford University. Arnold, J. E. (2001). The effect of thematic roles on pronoun use and frequency of reference continuation. Discourse Processes, 31(2), 137-162. Arnold, J. E., Bennetto, L., & Diehl, E. (2009). Reference production in young speakers with and without autism:Effects of discourse status and processing constraints. Cognition, 110(2), 131-46. Balogh, J. E. (2003). Pronouns, Prosody, and the Discourse Anaphora Weighting Approach (unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of California. Brehme, D. (2014). Perspectives on Personal Pronoun Reversal in Children with ASD:A Critical Review. Journal of European Psychology Students, 5(1), 31-37. Colle, L., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., & Van der Lely, H. (2008). Narrative Discourse in Adults with High-Functioning Autism or Asperger Syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(1), 28-40. Edelson, L. R. (2011). Cues to Pronominal Reference Resolution in Children with and Without Autism Spectrum Disorders (unpublished doctoral dissertation). Boston University. Gernsbacher, M. A., & Hargreaves, D. J. (1988). Accessing sentence participants:The advantage of first mention. Journal of Memory and Language, 27(6), 699-717. Grice, H.P. (1975). Logic and Conversation. In R. Cole & J. Morgan (Eds.), Syntax and Semantics:Speech Acts(pp.41-58). New York:Academic Press. Gundel, J. K., Hedberg, N., & Zacharski, R. (1993). Cognitive status and the form of referring expressions in discourse. Language, 69(2), 274-307. Hartshorne, J. K., Nappa, R., & Snedeker, J. (2015). Development of the first-mention bias. Journal of Child Language, 42(2), 423-446. Itzhak, I. & Baum, S. R. (2015). Misleading Bias-Driven Expectations in Referential Processing and the Facilitative Role of Contrastive Accent. Journal of Psycholinguist Research. 44(5), 623-650. Järvinen-Pasley, A., Peppé, S., King-Smith, G., & Heaton, P. (2008). The Relationship between Form and Function Level Receptive Prosodic Abilities in Autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(7), 1328-1340. Kertoy, M. K. (1991). Listening Comprehension for Sentences:the Accessibility of Referents for Pronouns as a Function of Age, Topic Continuity, and Pronoun Emphasis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 52(3), 344-353. Lombardo, M. V., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2010). Unravelling the paradox of the autistic self. Cognitive Science, 1(3), 393-403. Maratsos, M. P. (1973). The effects of stress on the understanding of pronominal coreference in children. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2(1), 1-8. Nieuwland, M. S., & van Berkum, J. J. A. (2006). Individual differences and contextual bias in pronoun resolution:Evidence from ERPs. Brain Research, 1118(1), 155-167. Novogrodsky, R. (2013). Subject pronoun used by children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD). Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 27(2), 85-93. Peppé, S., McCann, J., Gibbon, F., O'Hare, A., & Rutherford, M. (2007). Receptive and Expressive Prosodic Ability in Children with High-Functioning Autism. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 50(4), 1015. Snedeker, J., & Trueswell, John C. (2004). The developing constraints on parsing decisions:The role of lexical-biases and referential scenes in child and adult sentence processing. Cognitive Psychology, 49(3), 238-299. Solan, L. (1980). Contrastive Stress and Children's Interpretation of Pronouns. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 25(3), 688-698. Song, H. (2004). The Influence of Discourse Representation of Young Children's Pronoun Interpretation (unpublished doctoral dissertation). Seoul National University. Song, H., & Fisher, C. (2005). Who's ‘she’? Discourse prominence influences preschoolers' comprehension of pronouns. Journal of Memory and Language, 52(1), 29-57. Song, H., & Fisher, C. (2007). Discourse prominence effects on 2.5-year-old children's interpretation of pronouns. Lingua, 777(11), 1959-1987. Stevenson, R. J., Crawley, R. A., & Kleinman, D. (1994). Thematic roles, focus and the representation of events. Language and Cognitive Processes, 9(4), 473-592. Swaab, T. Y., Camblin, C. C, & Gordon, P. C. (2004). Electrophysiological evidence for reversed lexical repetition effects in language processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(5), 715-726. Terzi, A., Marinis, T., & Francis, K. (2016). The interface of syntax with pragmatics and prosody in children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism Development Disorder, 46(8), 2692-2706. Van Rij, J., Hollebrandse, B., & Hendriks, P. (2016). Children's eye gaze reveals their use of discourse context in object pronoun resolution(pp.267-293). Berlin/Boston:De Gruyter. 陈伟英, 皮姆·马克, 泰德·桑德斯.(2013).句法角色与话题性对实体突显性的影响.浙江大学学报(人文社会科学版), 43(1), 122-134. 李伟亚. (2009). 自闭症谱系障碍学生汉语句子理解过程的实验研究(博士学位论文). 华东师范大学, 上海. 缪小春. (1996). 影响代词加工的语义和语法因素研究. 心理学报, 28(4), 352-358. 邵敬敏. (主编)(2007). 现代汉语通论(pp.180-184). 第二版. 上海:上海教育出版社. 郑波, 王蓓, 杨玉芳. (2002). 韵律对指代歧义的解歧作用及其机制. 心理学报, 34(6), 567-572. |
|