Psychological Development and Education ›› 2020, Vol. 36 ›› Issue (5): 513-519.doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2020.05.01
DU Xuan1,2, LIN Jiayi1, CHEN Lijing1
CLC Number:
Bender, A., & Beller, S. (2014). Mapping spatial frames of reference onto time:A review of theoretical accounts and empirical findings. Cognition, 132(3), 342-382. Boroditsky, L. (2000). Metaphoric structuring:Understanding time through spatial metaphors. Cognition, 75(1), 1-28. Boroditsky, L. (2001). Does language shape thought?:Mandarin and English speakers' conceptions of time. Cognitive Psychology, 43(1), 1-22. Boroditsky, L., Fuhrman, O., & McCormick, K. (2011). Do English and Mandarin speakers think about time differently? Cognition, 118(1), 123-129. Bottini, R., Crepaldi, D., Casasanto, D., Crollen, V., & Collignon, O. (2015). Space and time in the sighted and blind. Cognition, 141, 67-72. Casasanto, D., & Boroditsky, L. (2008). Time in the mind:Using space to think about time. Cognition, 106(2), 579-593. Casasanto, D., Fotakopoulou, O., & Boroditsky, L. (2010). Space and time in the child's mind:Evidence for a cross-dimensional asymmetry. Cognitive Science, 34(3), 387-405. Chan, T. T., & Bergen, B. (2005). Writing direction influences spatial cognition. Paper presented at the Proceedings of the 27th annual conference of the cognitive science society. Chen, J. Y. (2007). Do Chinese and English speakers think about time differently? Failure of replicating Boroditsky (2001). Cognition, 104(2), 427-436. Ding, X., Feng, N., Cheng, X., Liu, H., & Fan, Z. (2015). Are past and future symmetric in mental time line? Frontiers in Psychology, 6(208). Giora R. (1999). On the priority of salient meanings:Studies of literal and figurative language. Journal of Pragmatics, 31(7), 919-929. Fuhrman, O., & Boroditsky, L. (2007). Mental time-lines follow writing direction:Comparing English and Hebrew speakers. Paper presented at the Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society. Fuhrman, O., & Boroditsky, L. (2010). Cross-cultural differences in mental representations of time:Evidence from an implicit nonlinguistic task. Cognitive Science, 34(8), 1430-1451. Li, H. (2017). Time on hands:deliberate and spontaneous temporal gestures by speakers of mandarin. Gesture, 16(3), 396-415. Lakoff, G. (1987). Women, fire, and dangerous things. Chicago:University of Chicago press, 77-90. Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago:University of Chicago press. Mashal, N., Faust, M., Hendler, T., (2005). The role of the right hemispherein processing nonsalient metaphorical meanings:application ofprincipal components analysis to fMRI data. Neuropsychologia, 43, 2084-2100. Nava, E., Rinaldi, L., Bulf, H., & Macchi Cassia, V. (2017). Visual and proprioceptive feedback differently modulate the spatial representation of number and time in children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 161, 161-177. Saj, A., Fuhrman, O., Vuilleumier, P., & Boroditsky, L. (2014). Patients with left spatial neglect also neglect the "left side" of time. Psychological Science, 25(1), 207-214. Santiago, J., Lupáñez, J., Pérez, E., & Funes, M. J. (2007). Time (also) flies from left to right. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(3), 512-516. Sell, A. J., & Kaschak, M. P. (2011). Processing time shifts affects the execution of motor responses. Brain and language, 117(1), 39-44. Torralbo, A., Santiago, J., & Lupiáñez, J. (2006). Flexible conceptual projection of time onto spatial frames of reference. Cognitive Science, 30(4), 745-757. Ulirich, R., Eikmeier, V., de la Vega, I., Fernández, S.R., Alex-Ruf, S., & Maienborn, C.(2012). With the past behind and the future ahead:Back-to front representation of past and future sentences. Memory & Cognition, 40(3), 483-495. Vallesi, A., Weisblatt, Y., Semenza, C., & Shaki, S. (2014). Cultural modulations of space-time compatibility effects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21(3), 666-669. Yang, W., & Sun, Y. (2016). English and mandarin speakers' mental representations of time. Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 14(2), 385-415. 陈栩茜, 张积家. (2011). 时间隐喻在汉语时间量词语义加工中的作用. 心理学报, 43(8), 863-877. 董奇. (1985). 小学儿童思维规则发展的实验研究. 心理发展与教育, 2 (3), 8-25. 郭晶晶, 赵婧超. (2017). 熟悉性对汉语隐喻义与本义加工机制的影响.心理科学, 40(6), 1302-1308. 金泓, 黄希庭. (2012). 时空隐喻研究的新问题:时间表征的左右方向性. 心理科学进展, 20(9), 1364-1371. 林崇德. (2008). 发展心理学. 北京:人民教育出版社, 271-272, 330-331. 王希杰. (1983). 汉语修辞学. 北京:北京出版社, 282-297. 周榕. (2003). 儿童时间隐喻能力发展趋势初探. 现代外语, 26(3),221-231. 朱智贤. (2003). 儿童心理学. 北京:人民教育出版社, 399-400. 朱智贤, 钱曼君, 吴凤岗, 林崇德. (1982). 小学生字词概念发展的研究. 心理科学通讯, 6(3), 23-29. |
|