Psychological Development and Education ›› 2018, Vol. 34 ›› Issue (6): 649-655.doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2018.06.02

Previous Articles     Next Articles

The Influence of Spatial Working Memory Load on Inhibition of Return Elicited by Central Gaze Cue

WANG Kaixuan, MIAO Yilin, CHE Xiaowei, LI Shouxin   

  1. School of Psychology, Shandong Normal University, Jinan 250358
  • Online:2018-11-15 Published:2018-12-27

Abstract: Two experiments were conducted to explore the influence of spatial working memory(SWM)load on inhibition of return (IOR) elicited by central gaze cue. In experiment 1, the cue relevant SWM load was manipulated by changing the similarity between the recognition and memory faces. The results showed that IOR was observed when the cue relevant SWM load was high, while no IOR was observed under low SWM load condition. Experiment 2 was conducted based on the condition that IOR was elicited by central gaze cue in experiment 1, and an additional cue irrelevant SWM task was presented before the gaze cue. The results showed that IOR was observed when the cue irrelevant SWM load was low, while IOR disappeared when the load was high. Taken together, our results revealed that IOR elicited by central gaze cue could be mediated by SWM resources. Whether IOR be elicited by central gaze cue may depend on the degree to which SWM resources was allocated to central gaze cue.

Key words: inhibition of return, spatial working memory, central gaze cue, load

CLC Number: 

  • B844
Bays, P. M., Catalao, R. F., & Husain, M. (2009). The precision of visual working memory is set by allocation of a shared resource. Journal of Vision, 9(10), 1-11.
Bjorklund, D. F., & Harnishfeger, K. K. (1990). The resources construct in cognitive development:Diverse sources of evidence and a theory of inefficient inhibition. Developmental Review, 10 (1), 48-71.
Castel, A. D., Pratt, J., & Craik, F. I. M. (2003). The role of spatial working memory in inhibition of return:Evidence from divided attention tasks. Perception & Psychophysics, 65(6), 970-981.
Chica, A. B., Bartolomeo, P., & Lupiáez, J. (2013). Two cognitive and neural systems for endogenous and exogenous spatial attention. Behavioural Brain Research, 237(1), 107-123.
Chou, W. L., & Yeh, S. L. (2008). Location-and object-based inhibition of return are affected by different kinds of working memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61(12), 1761-1768.
Cohen, M. A., Konkle, T., Rhee, J. Y., Nakayama, K., & Alvarez, G. A. (2014). Processing multiple visual objects is limited by overlap in neural channels. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111(24), 8955-8960.
Desimone, R., & Duncan, J. (1995). Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 18(1), 193-222.
Friesen, C. K., & Kingstone, A. (1998). The eyes have it! Reflexive orienting is triggered by nonpredictive gaze. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5(3), 490-495.
Frischen, A., Smilek, D., Eastwood, J. D., & Tipper, S. P. (2007). Inhibition of return in response to gaze cues:The roles of time course and fixation cue. Visual Cognition, 15(8), 881-895.
Frischen, A., & Tipper, S. P. (2004). Orienting attention via observed gaze shift evokes longer term inhibitory effects:implications for social interactions, attention, and memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology:General, 133(4), 516-533.
Fuentes, L. J., Vivas, A. B., & Humphreys, G. W. (1999). Inhibitory mechanisms of attentional networks:Spatial and semantic inhibitory processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Human Perception and Performance, 25(4), 1114-1126.
Jiang, Y. V., Lee, H. J., Asaad, A., & Remington, R. (2016). Similarity effects in visual working memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23(2), 476-482.
Klein, R. (1988). Inhibitory tagging system facilitates visual search. Nature, 334(6181), 430-431.
Langton, S. R. H. & Bruce, V. (1999). Reflexive visual orienting in response to the social attention of others. Visual Cognition, 6(5), 541-567.
McDonnell, G. P., & Dodd, M. D. (2013). Examining the influence of a spatially irrelevant working memory load on attentional allocation. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Human Perception and Performance, 39(4),933-940.
Michael, D., Stefan, V. S., & Andrew, H. (2009). Novelty is not always the best policy:Inhibition of return and facilitation of return as a function of visual task. Psychological Science, 20(3), 333-339.
Müller, H. J., & Rabbitt, P. M. (1989). Reflexive and voluntary orienting of visual attention:Time course of activation and resistance to interruption. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Human Perception and Performance, 15(2), 315-330.
Posner, M. I., & Cohen, Y. (1984). Components of visual orienting. Attention and performance X:ontrol of language processes, 32(4), 531-556.
Silk, T. J., Bellgrove, M. A., Wrafter, P., Mattingley, J. B., & Cunnington, R. (2010). Spatial working memory and spatial attention rely on common neural processes in the intraparietal sulcus. NeuroImage, 53(2), 718-724.
Vivas, A. B., Liaromati, I., Masoura, E., & Chatzikallia, K. (2010). Re-examining the contribution of visuospatial working memory to inhibition of return. Psychological Research, 74(6), 524-531.
Woodman, G. F., & Luck, S. J. (2004). Visual search is slowed when visuospatial working memory is occupied. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11(2), 269-274.
Yantis, S., & Hillstrom, A. P. (1994). Stimulus-driven attentional capture:Evidence from equiluminant visual objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Human Perception and Performance, 20(1), 95-107.
Zhang, Y., & Zhang, M. (2011). Spatial working memory load impairs manual but not saccadic inhibition of return. Vision research, 51(1), 147-153.
陈衍, 陈新宇, 田颖, 白学军. (2014). 视空间工作记忆负荷对返回抑制影响的年老化研究. 现代预防医学, 41(20), 3738-3740.
顾本柏. (2011). 客体相似性表征的认知和发展机制研究(博士学位论文). 西南大学, 重庆.
秦速励, 沈政. (2001). 面孔识别的"特殊性". 心理科学, 24(5), 604-605.
徐岩, 张亚旭, 周晓林. (2003). 面孔加工的认知神经科学研究:回顾与展望. 心理科学进展, 11(1), 35-43.
杨华海, 赵晨. (1998). 外源性视觉选择性注意的时空特征. 心理学报, 30(2), 136-142.
张智君, 赵亚军, 占琪涛. (2011). 注视方向的知觉对注视追随行为的影响. 心理学报, 43(7), 726-738.
赵静, 潘毅. (2010). 工作记忆内容对空间返回抑制的影响. 心理学探新, 30(115), 42-46.
赵亚军, 张智君. (2009). 眼睛注视线索提示效应:内源性注意还是外源性注意? 心理学报, 41(12), 1133-1142.
[1] CHEN Bizhong, ZHENG Xue, SUN Xiaojun. The Relationship between Problematic Social Media Use and Online Social Anxiety: The Roles of Social Media Cognitive Overload and Dispositional Mindfulness [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2023, 39(5): 743-751.
[2] YU Xiao, ZHANG Heyi, QI Yue, CHEN Yinghe, LIU Haoning, ZHAO Yuxi, QIAO Xuewen. The Role of Inhibitory Control in the Development of Analogical Mapping: Cognitive Load Matters [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2023, 39(1): 1-11.
[3] WANG Yanqing, YANG Xiaomeng, ZHAO Tingting, GAO Chunying, ZHANG Yang, ZHAO Qingbai, WANG Fuxing, HU Xiang'en. Does Segmentation Facilitate Video Learning? [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2022, 38(4): 600-608.
[4] REN Yi, HUANG Silin. The Mechanisms of Poverty Impedes Children's Executive Function [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2022, 38(1): 134-143.
[5] CHEN Lijun, LIU Limin, LIN Yueyang, ZHANG Lingyan. Research on Cognitive Load of Graphic Processing in Virtual Reality Learning Environment [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2021, 37(5): 619-627.
[6] YUAN Juanjuan, YANG Yang, ZHENG Zhiwei, LIU Pingping. Picture-text Horizontal Layout Enhances Recognition of Unfamiliar Words: Evidence from 8-year-old Children and Adults [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2021, 37(4): 525-538.
[7] LIU Qinxue, ZHANG Juyuan, LIN Yue. The Relationship between College Students' Smartphone Addiction and Inhibitory Control: The Moderating Roles of Phone Position and Cognitive Load [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2021, 37(2): 257-265.
[8] WU Wenchun, CHEN Jiaqian, LIU Chang. Time Course of Feature Processing for 3-D Graphics in Object and Spatial Working Memory [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2019, 35(5): 513-521.
[9] WANG Shu, YIN Yue, WANG Ting, LIU Guofang, LUO Junlong. The Learning Performance and Flow Experience in Instructional Design, Based on Cognitive Load Theory [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2019, 35(3): 329-337.
[10] CHEN Chunyu, LIAN Shuailei, SUN Xiaojun, CHAI Huanyou, ZHOU Zongkui. The Effect of Social Network Sites Addiction on Adolescents' Depression: Mediating Role of Cognitive Overload and Core Self-evaluation [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2018, 34(2): 210-218.
[11] WU Wenchun, SUN Yueliang, XU Xue. The Storage Mechanism of Object and Spatial Working Memory for the 3-D Graphs [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2017, 33(2): 145-152.
[12] WANG Li, LI Shouxin, ZHANG Qian. The Effect of the Focality of Processing and Ongoing Task Load on Prospective Memory for Undergraduates with Different Cognitive Styles [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2016, 32(2): 149-157.
[13] DAI Man, LIU Ru-de, DI Miao-ci, MA Xue-ying, XU Le. The Effects of Working Memory Load and Automatic Retrieval on Elementary School Students Mental Addition Strategy Execution [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2014, 30(4): 420-426.
[14] WU Wen-chun. A Study on the Storage of Object and Spatial Working Memory for the Graphs with Features of Double Dimensions [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2013, 29(3): 247-254.
[15] LUO Liang, LIN Chong-de, CHEN Guang. Effects of Spatial and Object-based Attention Interference on Spatial and Object Working Memory [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2010, 26(6): 561-568,576.
Viewed
Full text


Abstract

Cited

  Shared   
  Discussed   
No Suggested Reading articles found!