Psychological Development and Education ›› 2016, Vol. 32 ›› Issue (2): 149-157.doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2016.02.03

Previous Articles     Next Articles

The Effect of the Focality of Processing and Ongoing Task Load on Prospective Memory for Undergraduates with Different Cognitive Styles

WANG Li, LI Shouxin, ZHANG Qian   

  1. School of Psychology, Shandong Normal University, Jinan 250014
  • Online:2016-03-15 Published:2016-03-15

Abstract: The present study aimed to investigate how the focality of processing and ongoing task load affect the event-based prospective memory (EBPM) for undergraduates with different cognitive styles. This study contained two experiments. Experiment 1 examined the effect of the focality of processing and the matching in the types of processing between the prospective memory task and ongoing task on EBPM for field-independent (FI) and field-dependent (FD) undergraduates. Experiment 2 investigated how ongoing task load and the focality of processing affected the EBPM for FI and FD undergraduates. The results showed that, (1) the performance in focal processing condition was better than that in nonfocal processing condition, which was not affected by the change of the ongoing task load; (2) under conditions of high ongoing task load and nonfocal processing, the performance for FI undergraduates was significantly better than that for FD ones, which was not affected by the matching in the types of processing between the prospective memory task and ongoing task. The present study supports the multiprocess theory.

Key words: cognitive style, focal processing, ongoing task load, event-based prospective memory, TAP effect

CLC Number: 

  • B844
Abney, D. H., McBride, D. M., & Petrella, S. N. (2013). Interactive effects in transfer-appropriate processing for evented-based prospective memory:The roles of efforts, ongoing task, and PM cue properties. Memory & Cognition, 41(7), 1032-1045.
Blowers, G. H., & O'CONNOR, K. (1978). Relation of eye movements to errors on the rod-and-frame test. Perceptual and motor skills, 46(3), 719-725.
Brewer, G. A., Knight, J. B., Marsh, R. L., & Unsworth, N. (2010). Individual differences in event-based prospective memory:Evidence for multiple processes supporting cue detection. Memory & Cognition, 38(3), 304-311.
Cohen, A. L., Jaudas, A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2008). Number of cues influences the cost of remembering to remember. Memory & cognition, 36(1), 149-156.
Cona, G., Bisiacchi, P. S., & Moscovitch, M. (2013). The effects of focal and nonfocal cues on the neural correlates of prospective memory:insights from ERPs. Cerebral Cortex, 24(10), 2630-2646.
Cronbach, L. J. (1990). Essentials of psychological testing. 5th ed. New York:happer and Row publishers, 206-218.
Einstein, G. O., & McDaniel, M. A. (2005). Prospective memory:multiple retrieval processes. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14(6), 286-290.
Einstein, G. O., McDaniel, M. A., Thomas, R., Mayfield, S., Shank, H., Morrisette, N., & Breneiser, J. (2005). Mulitiple processes in prospective memory retrieval:Factors determining monitoring versus spontaneous retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology:General, 134(3), 327-342.
Einstein, G. O., McDaniel, M. A., Williford, C. L., Pagan, J. L., & Dismukes, R. (2003). Forgetting of intentions in demanding situations is rapid. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Applied, 9(3), 147-162.
Goode, P. E., Goddard, P. H., & Pascual-Leone, J. (2002). Event-related potentials index cognitive style differences during a serial order recall task. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 43(2), 123-140.
Jia, S., Zhang, Q., & Li, S. (2014). Field dependence-independence modulates the efficiency of filtering out irrelevant information in a visual working memory task. Neuroscience, 278, 136-143.
Kliegel, M., Martin, M., McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. (2004). Importance effects on performance in event-based prospective memory tasks. Memory, 12(5), 553-561.
Kliegel, M., & Jäger, T. (2006). The influence of negative emotions on prospective memory:A review and new data. International Journal of Computational Cognition, 4(1), 1-17.
Loft, S., & Yeo, G. (2007). An investigation into the resource requirements of event-based prospective memory. Memory & Cognition, 35(2), 263-264.
Marsh, R. L., Hicks, J. L., & Hancock, T. W. (2000). On the interaction of ongoing cognitive activity and the nature of an event-based intention. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 14(7), S29-S41.
Maylor, E. A. (1996). Age-related impairment in an event-based prospective-memory task. Psychology and Aging, 11(1), 74-78.
McBride, D. M., & Abney, D. H. (2012). A comparison of transferappropriate processing and multiprocess frameworks for prospective memory performance. Experimental Psychology, 59(4), 190-198.
McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. (2000). Strategic and automatic processes in prospective memory retrieval:A multiprocess framework. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 14(7), 127-144.
McGann, D., Ellis, J., & Milne, A. (2003). Conceptual and perceptual processing in prospective remembering. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology,15(1), 19-42.
McNerney, M. W., & West, R. (2007). An imperfect relationship between prospective memory and the prospective interference effect. Memory & Cognition, 35(2), 275-282.
Meier, B., & Graf, P. (2000). Transfer appropriate processing for prospective memory tests. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 14(7), 11-27.
Meier, B., Zimmermann, T. D., & Perrig, W. J. (2006). Retrieval experience in prospective memory:Strategic monitoring and spontaneous retrieval. Memory, 14(7), 872-889.
Norman, D. A., & Bobrow, D. G. (1975). On data-limited and resource-limited processes. Cognitive psychology, 7(1), 44-64.
Scullin, M. K., McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. (2010). Control of cost in prospective memory:Evidence for spontaneous retrieval processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Learning, Memory, and Cognition,36(1), 190-203.
Scullin, M. K., McDaniel, M. A., Shelton, J. T., & Lee, J. H. (2010). Focal/nonfocal cue effects in prospective memory:Monitoring difficulty or different retrieval processes? Journal of Experimental Psychology:Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36(3), 736-749.
West, R., & Craik, F. I. M. (2001). Influences on the efficiency of prospective memory in younger and older adults. Psychology and Aging, 16(4), 682-696.
Wickens, C. D. (1991). Processing resources and attention. Multiple-task performance, 3-34.
Witkin, H. A., & Goodenough, D. R. (1981). Cognitive styles, essence and origins:Field dependence and field independence. New York:International Universities Press.
贾文华. (2009). 问题解决模式与场依存性认知方式的相关研究. 宁波大学学报, 31(4), 60-64.
李寿欣, 丁兆叶, 张利增. (2005). 认知方式与线索特征对前瞻记忆的影响. 心理学报,37(3), 320-327.
李寿欣, 董立达, 宫大志. (2008). 注意状态, 认知方式与前瞻记忆的 TAP 效应. 心理学报, 40(11), 1149-1157.
李寿欣, 宋艳春. (2006). 不同认知方式中小学生的前瞻记忆的研究. 心理发展与教育, 2, 18-22.
李寿欣, 周颖萍. (2006). 个体认知方式与材料复杂性对视空间工作记忆的影响. 心理学报, 38(4), 523-531.
孟庆茂, 常建华. (1988). 关于《 镶嵌图形测验》 评分方法及部分常模的修订. 见:谢斯骏, 张厚粲, 认知方式——一个人格维度的实验研究. 北京:北京师范大学出版社, 278-280.
王丽娟, 吴韬, 邱文威, 叶媛, 马薇薇, 李霓. (2010). 青少年基于事件的前瞻记忆:认知方式和情绪. 心理科学, 33(5), 1244-1247.
王永跃, 葛列众, 王健. (2010). 不同难度背景条件下前瞻记忆任务的实验研究. 心理科学, 33(3), 612-615.
闫春平, 孙丽君, 杨世昌, 杜卫, 周丹丹. (2013). 认知闭合需要对个体前瞻记忆的影响. 中国临床心理学杂志, 21(3), 360-362.
张斌. (2013). 不同工作记忆负荷对形状干扰子注意捕获的影响. 山东师范大学学报, 58(4), 120-124.
[1] HU Weiping, ZHAO Xiaomei, JIA Peiyuan, CHEN Yinghe. The Effect of Learn to Think Online Program on Creativity of Primary School Students: Moderating Effects of Cognitive Style [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2017, 33(3): 257-264.
[2] MA Junpeng, YE Zhuoer, LIN Yi, GAO Hongwei, HUANG Ping, LIN Huiyan, XU Huan, YANG Nan, JIN Hua. Does Neural Responses to Categorization Depend on Cognitive Style: An Evidence from fMRI Study [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2016, 32(2): 139-148.
[3] CHENG Lifang, HU Weiping, JIA Xiaojuan. Cognitive Inhibition and Artistic Creativity: The Moderating Effect of Cognitive Style [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2015, 31(3): 287-295.
[4] CHEN Ying-he, HAO Jia-jia. Individual Differences in Reflective-Impulsive Cognitive Style: Children’s Problem Solving and Metacognition [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2011, 27(3): 241-246.
[5] Han, Xiu, Pei-yang-hong. The Effects of the College Students’ Intelligence and Cognitive Styles on Implicit Sequence Learning [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2010, 26(1): 48-53.
[6] KANG Cheng, ZHOU Ai-bao. The Effect of Representational Mode of Information and Personality Traits of Learners on the Learning in Multimedia Environment [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2009, 25(1): 83-91.
[7] WANG Jing, CHEN Ying-he. The Influence of Cognitive Style in Cooperative Learning Group to It’s Problem Solving [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2008, 24(2): 102-107.
[8] LI Shou-xin, SONG Yan-chun. Research on the Prospective Memory of Students with Different Cognitive Styles [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2006, 22(2): 18-22.
[9] WU Guo-lai, WO Jian-zhong, BAI Xue-jun, SHEN De-li. The Difference of Implicit Sequence Learning in 11-year-old Children Between Cognitive Styles [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2006, 22(1): 39-42.
[10] LI Shou-xin, CHEN Hong-min. Influence of Cognitive Styles and Subjects on Reading Transfer [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2002, 18(4): 52-55.
[11] LI Li-hong, CHANG Fong-jin. The Influence of Primary Schoolers’ Field Independent Style and field Dependent Style on the Effects of Trial-teaching and Normal-teaching [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2002, 18(2): 55-59.
[12] . The Effects of Students Cognigive Styles on Article Reading Comprehension [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2001, 17(2): 31-35.
[13] 杨卫星, 王学臣, 张梅玲. The Influence of Awareness of Isomorphic Problems and Students’ Cognitive Style on Geometry Problem-solving Transfer [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2000, 16(2): 17-21,27.
Viewed
Full text


Abstract

Cited

  Shared   
  Discussed   
No Suggested Reading articles found!