心理发展与教育 ›› 2005, Vol. 21 ›› Issue (1): 21-25.

• 认知与社会性发展 • 上一篇    下一篇

从儿童赌博任务看热执行功能的发展

李红, 高山, 白俊杰   

  1. 西南师范大学心理学院, 重庆, 400715
  • 出版日期:2005-01-15 发布日期:2005-01-15
  • 作者简介:李红(1965- ),男,西南师大学心理学院教授.
  • 基金资助:
    中国科学院知识创新工程重要方向项目"情绪调节机制对儿童环境适应与创新的影响"(项目批准号:KSCX2-SW-221)一级子课题"儿童情绪与认知过程的交互作用"的部分工作.

Development of “Hot” Executivc Function through Children’s Gambling Task

LI Hong, GAO Shan, BAI Jun-jie   

  1. School of Psychology, Southwest China Normal University, Key Laboratory of Basic Psychology of Chongqing, Beibei Chongqing 400715
  • Online:2005-01-15 Published:2005-01-15

摘要: 为考察热执行功能的发展,本研究通过儿童赌博任务检测了60名3~4岁儿童(男女各半)的情感决策。实验结果表明,3岁和4岁儿童在此任务上存在显著的年龄差异,4岁组在各试验组的分数呈上升趋势,他们在试验组2、尤其在试验组4和5中选择有利纸牌的次数显著多于3岁儿童,这反映出情感决策在3~4岁期间发展迅速。此外,儿童在此任务上的成绩存在很多可变性,可能掩盖了性别差异,因而,尽管男孩在最后两个试验组的成绩优于女孩,但性别差异在统计上并不显著。

关键词: 执行功能, 热执行功能, 冷执行功能, 情感决策

Abstract: Affective decision-making was studied in 60 children at 2 ages (3 and 4 years) using the Children's Gambling Task,to examine the development of hot executive function.Results showed a significant age difference between 3-and 4-year-olds.Scores increased across blocks for 4-year-olds,and 4-year-olds made more advantageous choices than 3-year-olds on three blocks,especially the last two blocks.This indicated that affective decision 2 making develops rapidly between 3 and 4 years.On the other hand,a lot of variability in children's performance on this task migh have obscured sex effects.Therefore,sex difference was not statistically significant,though boys did better than girls on the last two blocks.

Key words: executive function, hot executive function, cool executive function, affective decision-making, orbitofrontal cortex (OFC)

中图分类号: 

  • B844.1
[1] Zelazo P D,Muller U.Executive function in typical and atypical development.In:Goswami U ed.Handbook of Childhood Cognitive Development.Oxford:Blackwell,2002.445-469.
[2] Miller E K,Cohen J D.An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function.Annual Review of Neuroscience,2001,24:167-202.
[3] Frye D,Zelazo P D,Palfai T.Theory of mind and rule-based reasoning.Cognitive Development,1995,10:483-527.
[4] Kerr A,Zelazo P D.Development of "hot" executive function:The Children's Gambling Task.Brain and Cognition,in press.
[5] Peters E,Slovic P.The springs of affection:Affective and analytical information processing in choice.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,2000,26:1465-1475.
[6] Overman W H,Bachevalier J,Schuhmann E,Ryan P.Cognitive gender differnces in very young children parallel biologically based cognitive gender differences in monkeys.Behavioral Neuroscience,1996,110:673-684.
[7] Clark A S,Goldman-Rakic P S.Gonadal hormones influence the emergence of cortical function in nonhuman primates.Behavioral Neuroscience,1989,103:1287-1295.
[8] Reavis R,Overman W H.Adult sex differences on a decisionmaking task previously shown to depend on the orbital prefrontal cortex.Behavioral Neuroscience,2001,115:196-206.
[9] Rolls E T.The orbitofrontal cortex and reward.Cerebral Cortex (Special Issue:The mysterious orbitofronal cortex),2000,10:284-294.
[10] Damasio A.Descartes' error:emotion,reason,and the human brain.New York:Grosset/Putnam,1994.
[11] Bechara A,Damasio H,Tranel D,Damasio A.Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy.Science,1997,275:1293-1295.
[12] Zelazo P D,Frye D.Cognitive complexity and control:Ⅱ.the development of executive function.Current Directions in Psychological Science,1998,7:121-126.
[13] Zelazo P D,Frye D,Rapus T.An age-related dissociation between knowing rules and using them.Cognitive Development,1996,11:37-63.
[1] 颜志强, 周可, 曾晓, 徐惠, 朱晓倩, 张娟. 学前期儿童执行功能与攻击性行为的关系:认知共情的中介作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(6): 788-797.
[2] 刘钰, 苏彦捷, 曹春梅, 丁宇, 樊煜. 社区老年人执行功能和加工速度与运动能力的关系[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2022, 38(6): 769-777.
[3] 王颖, 朱海东, 张馨艺, 肖沿. 学龄前留守儿童亲子依恋对执行功能的影响:祖孙依恋的调节作用和中介作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2022, 38(2): 195-206.
[4] 白荣, 闫嵘, 王千, 李叶, 邢淑芬. 学前儿童执行功能与问题行为的关系:情境和性别的特异性[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2022, 38(1): 35-44.
[5] 任屹, 黄四林. 贫困损害儿童执行功能的作用机制[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2022, 38(1): 134-143.
[6] 王兴华, 洪慧芳, 朱瑞玲. 2~3岁幼儿执行功能与母子互动的关系:幼儿气质的调节作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2021, 37(6): 784-791.
[7] 占淑玮, 杨宁, 赵必华. 留守学前儿童接受性语言能力与社会退缩的关系:有调节的中介模型[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2021, 37(6): 834-844.
[8] 慕德芳, 陈英和. 小学高年级学生元认知监测对认知操作输出的影响:执行功能的调节作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2021, 37(3): 305-312.
[9] 韩燕, 徐芬. 儿童青少年时期加工速度和执行功能在流体智力发展中的作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2020, 36(4): 394-405.
[10] 王静梅, 张义宾, 郑晨烨, 卢英俊, 秦金亮. 3~6岁儿童执行功能子成分发展的研究[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2019, 35(1): 1-10.
[11] 田丽丽, 周欣, 康丹, 徐晶晶, 李正清. 5~6岁不同数学能力水平儿童的执行功能差异研究[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2016, 32(1): 9-16.
[12] 吴慧中, 王明怡. 2~3.5岁儿童执行功能发展特点及其言语能力的影响[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2015, 31(6): 654-660.
[13] 苏彦捷, 于晶. 执行功能与心理理论关系的元分析: 抑制控制和灵活转换的作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2015, 31(1): 51-61.
[14] 张乾一, 文萍. 3岁幼儿抑制和工作记忆的可分离性研究[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2013, 29(3): 238-246.
[15] 黄翯青, 苏彦捷. 共情的毕生发展:一个双过程的视角[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2012, 28(4): 434-441.
Viewed
Full text


Abstract

Cited

  Shared   
  Discussed   
No Suggested Reading articles found!