心理发展与教育 ›› 2012, Vol. 28 ›› Issue (6): 588-594.

• 论文 • 上一篇    下一篇

幼儿同伴关系发展的稳定性

刘少英1, 王芳2, 朱瑶2   

  1. 1. 浙江理工大学心理系, 浙江杭州310018;
    2. 浙师大幼师幼儿园, 浙江杭州310012
  • 出版日期:2012-11-15 发布日期:2012-11-15
  • 通讯作者: 刘少英,E-mail:syliu@zstu.edu.cn E-mail:syliu@zstu.edu.cn
  • 基金资助:
    教育部重点课题(DBA080051);浙江省级实验教学示范中心建设单位资助(浙教办高教[2010]183号)

Stability of Preschooler’s Peer Relationship

LIU Shao-ying1, WANG Fang2, ZHU Yao2   

  1. 1. Department of Psychology, Zhejiang Sci-tech University, Hangzhou 310018, China;
    2. Affiliated Kindergartens, Zhejiang Normal School, Hangzhou 310012, China
  • Online:2012-11-15 Published:2012-11-15

摘要: 研究采用同伴提名法,从2004年到2008年对三个自然班幼儿(71名)群体水平的同伴关系分别在三个时间点开始从小班到大班的追踪,考察学前期幼儿同伴关系连续变量的稳定性,结果发现:(1)幼儿同伴接纳、同伴拒绝、社会偏好和社会影响的稳定性受年龄和时间间隔的影响,在中班与大班之间均存在中等相关,而社会偏好和同伴接纳在小班与中班也存在中等相关,社会偏好在小班与大班也存在中等相关;(2)幼儿社会偏好发展模式经聚类分析为4组:一般偏好稳定组、高偏好稳定组、一般到高偏好变化组和低偏好变化组,64.79%的幼儿社会偏好发展保持稳定;(3)幼儿从小班起,女孩比男孩一直更少受到同伴拒绝,更多受到同伴偏爱。总体而言,学前期幼儿同伴关系发展具有中等稳定性,中班是稳定性发展的重要时期。

关键词: 幼儿, 同伴关系, 同伴提名, 稳定性

Abstract: The stability of peer relationship was a fundamental question that was inherently appealing to developmental psychologists.In preschool years,group relations in peers began and peer relationship consolidated. The present longitudinal study examined the stability of preschooler's peer relationship.By studying the stability of peer relationship,we gained insight into questions regarding the degree to which the stability of continuous variables in peer relations,and especially whether the social preference development changed across early childhood. The author determined a sample of 71 preschool-aged children(40 boys,31 girls;M age=46.42 months), who came from three classes in different time points that the first class was from 2004 to 2006,the second class was from 2005 to 2007,and the third class was from 2006 to 2008.Children completed peer nomination for 3 successive years from grade 1 to grade 3. Results indicated that long-term stability of four continuous dimensions of peer relationship(peer acceptance, peer rejection,social preference,and social impact)was influenced by 2 factors:children's age and the length of the time interval.Four continuous dimensions of peer relationship had moderate stability(r=0.41~0.59) between grade 2 and grade 3.Social preference had moderate stability(r=0.31~0.59)from grade 1 to grade 3. Peer acceptance had moderate stability(r=0.37)between grade 1 and grade 2.Other dimensions had no significant correlations among three grades.Grade 2 was the transition of peer relationship in preschool years. Using a clustering technique,the study identified four groups of children with distinct longitudinal profiles of social preference development-a stable average group(64.79%),an unstable popular group(15.49%),an unstable unpopular group(12.68%),and an unstable group from average to popular(7.04%).The result indicated that 64.79% children'social preference kept stable for three years. In this experiment,the effect of gender on social preference was significant(F(1,70)=11.970,p<0.05), while the other two effects were not significant.So girls were more popular than boys consistently in three years. There was no significant gender and age difference on social impact. Conclusions:The current study provided evidence for moderate stability of peer status in preschool years,and girls were more popular than boys.The result suggested that grade 2 was the key time for the stability of peer relationship.

Key words: Preschooler, Peer Relationship, Peer nomination, Stability

中图分类号: 

  • B844.1
[1] Baines,E.,&Blatchford,P.(2009).Sex differences in the structure and stability of children's playground social networks and their overlap with friendship relations.British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 27(3),743-760.
[2] Benenson,J.,Apostoleris,N.,&Parnass,J.(1998).The Organization of Children's Same-Sex Peer Relationships.New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development,1998(81),5-23.
[3] Brendgen,M.,Vitaro,F.,Bukowski,W.M.,Doyle,A.B.,& Markiewicz,D.(2001).Developmental profiles of peer social preference over the course of elementary school:associations with trajectories of externalizing and internalizing behavior.Developmental Psychology,37(3),308-320.
[4] Bukowski,W.M.,&Adams,R.(2005).Peer relationships and psychopathology:Markers,moderators,mediators,mechanisms,and meanings.Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology,34 (1),3-10.
[5] Cillessen,A.H.N.,Bukowski,W.M.,&Haselager,G.J.T. (2000).Stability of sociometric categories.New directions for child and adolescent development,2000(88),75-93.
[6] Fabes,R.A.,Martin,C.L.,&Hanish,L.D.(2004).The Next 50 Years:Considering Gender as a Context for Understanding Young Children's Peer Relationships.Merrill Palmer Quarterly Journal of Developmental Psychology,50(3),260-273.
[7] Greenman,P.S.,Schneider,B.H.,&Tomada,G.(2009).Stability and change in patterns of peer rejection:Implications for children's academic performance over time.School Psychology International,30(2),163-183.
[8] Hay,D.F.(2005).Early peer relations and their impact on children's development.In.RE Tremblay,RG Barr,&RDeV Peters,(éds).Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development(online).Montréal,Québec:Centre of Excellence for Early Childhood Development.Récupéréle,1-6.
[9] Hay,D.F.,Payne,A.,&Chadwick,A.(2004).Peer relations in childhood.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,45(1),84-108.
[10] Howes,C.,&Phillipsen,L.(1998).Continuity in children's relations with peers.Social Development,7(3),340-349.
[11] Jiang,X.L.,&Cillessen,A.H.N.(2005).Stability of continuous measures of sociometric status:A meta-analysis.Developmental Review,25(1),1-25.
[12] Ladd,G.W.(1999).Peer relationships and social competence during early and middle childhood.Annual Review of Psychology,50(1),333-359.
[13] Laine,K.,Neitola,M.,Auremaa,J.,&Laakkonen,E.(2010).Longitudinal study on the co-occurrence of peer problems at daycare centre,in preschool and first grade of school.Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research,54(5),471-485.
[14] Laursen,B.,&Hoff,E.(2006).Person-centered and variable-centered approaches to longitudinal data.Merrill-Palmer Quarterly,52 (3),377-389.
[15] Martinsen,H.,Narland,T.,&Vereijken,B.(2010).Observation-based descriptions of social status in the pre-school.Early Child Development and Care,180(9),1231-1241.
[16] Meijs,N.,Cillessen,A.H.N.,Scholte,R.H.J.,Segers,E.,& Spijkerman,R.(2009).Social intelligence and academic achievement as predictors of adolescent popularity.Journal of Youth and Adolescence,39(1),62-72.
[17] Mouton,J.S.,Blake,R.R.,&Fruchter,B.(1955).The reliability of sociometric measures.Sociometry,18(1),7-48.
[18] Poteat,G.M.,Ironsmith,M.,&Bullock,J.(1986).The classification of preschool children's sociometric status.Early Childhood Research Quarterly,1(4),349-360.
[19] Rubin,K.H.,Bukowski,W.,&Parker,J.G.(2003).Peer interactions, relationships,and groups.Handbook of child psychology.Vol3.Social,Emotional,and Personality Development,6th.ed.,619-700.
[20] Shin,N.,Vaughn,B.E.,Kim,M.,Krzysik,L.,Bost,K.K.,McBride,B.,et al.(2011).Longitudinal analyses of a hierarchical model of peer social competence for preschool children structural fidelity and external correlates.Merrill-Palmer Quarterly,57(1),73-103.
[21] Vitaro,F.,Tremblay,R.E.,Gagnon,C.,&Boivin,M.(1992).Peer rejection from kindergarten to Grade 2:Outcomes,correlates, and prediction.Merrill-Palmer Quarterly:Journal of Developmental Psychology,38(3),382-400.
[22] Walker,S.(2009).Sociometric stability and the behavioral correlates of peer acceptance in early childhood.The Journal of genetic psychology, 170(4),339-358.
[23] 李艳菊,姜勇.(2008).3~6岁幼儿同伴交往能力的结构与发展特点.幼儿教育(教育科学版),(7),70-73.
[24] 刘红云.(2007).如何描述发展趋势的差异:潜变量混合增长模型.心理科学进展,15(3),539-544.
[25] 庞丽娟.(1994).同伴提名法与幼儿同伴交往的研究.心理发展与教育,1(1),18-22.
[26] 邹晓燕,李英玉,黄晓梅.(2006).幼儿同伴关系发展特点与教育.幼儿教育(教育科学版),(7),72-76.
[1] 高子惠, 焦雨, 王曦, 刘肖岑. 电子绘本文字的动静态呈现方式对幼儿阅读体验和学习效果的影响[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(6): 817-824.
[2] 王英杰, 栾金鑫, 李燕. 母亲养育压力与幼儿焦虑的关系:亲子冲突的中介作用和母亲正念的调节作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(5): 702-709.
[3] 张文洁, 范增, 黎蕾, 刘毅, 钟毅平. 接受对象的常规行为与资源需求对5~6岁幼儿分享行为的影响[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(4): 465-472.
[4] 牛湘, 冉光明. 同伴关系与幼儿问题行为关系的三水平元分析[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(4): 473-487.
[5] 刘双, 李梅, 冯晓杭, 张向葵. 幼儿掌控动机与能力关系的纵向研究:任务类型与气质的调节作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(2): 161-172.
[6] 王英杰, 张刈, 张美霞, 李燕. 母亲正念与幼儿问题行为的关系:正念养育和亲子关系的链式中介作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(2): 184-191.
[7] 张彩, 江伊茹, 朱成伟, 邵婷婷, 王海涛, 陈福美. 学校归属感与青少年手机依赖的关系:学习焦虑的中介效应与同伴关系的调节效应[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2022, 38(6): 848-858.
[8] 刘晓洁, 李燕. 母亲感知的共同教养对幼儿行为问题的影响:一个有调节的中介模型[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2022, 38(5): 626-634.
[9] 张和颐, 洪秀敏. 家庭认知环境与0~3岁婴幼儿发展的关系:有调节的中介效应[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2022, 38(2): 178-185.
[10] 许郡婷, 鲍未, 罗俊龙. 不稳定控制感剥夺损害工作记忆刷新功能[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2022, 38(1): 1-9.
[11] 王薇, 贡德英, 程春, 陈科, 李永鑫, 闫晶晶. 5~7岁自闭症谱系障碍幼儿数感的特点[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2021, 37(6): 818-825.
[12] 刘田田, 李燕, 李有嘉, 姜新苗. 父母协同教养与学前儿童社会行为的关系:亲子关系和同胞关系的链式中介作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2021, 37(5): 638-647.
[13] 周思妤, 王艺卓, 李晓巍. 幼儿园新教师学历与离职意向的关系:入职适应、工作满意度的多重中介作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2021, 37(5): 675-682.
[14] 贺晓玲, 陈俊, 刘灵, 张甜. 3~5岁幼儿职业声望垂直空间、大小双重隐喻发展[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2021, 37(4): 457-464.
[15] 李占星, 朱莉琪. “它”是所有者吗?幼儿对所有权主体的理解[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2021, 37(4): 465-471.
Viewed
Full text


Abstract

Cited

  Shared   
  Discussed   
No Suggested Reading articles found!