心理发展与教育 ›› 2022, Vol. 38 ›› Issue (5): 749-760.doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2022.05.16

• 理论探讨与进展 • 上一篇    

儿童时间认知研究范式:困境与超越

余习德   

  1. 广东技术师范大学教育科学与技术学院, 广州 510665
  • 发布日期:2022-09-14
  • 通讯作者: 余习德 E-mail:yucider19872@126.com
  • 基金资助:
    广东省哲学社会科学“十三五”规划学科共建项目“时间流逝感的元认知研究”(GD20XXL05)。

Research Paradigms of Children's Time Cognition: Dilemma and Transcendence

YU Xide   

  1. School of Education Science and Technology, Guangdong Polytechnic Normal University, Guangzhou 510665
  • Published:2022-09-14

摘要: 西方实证主义导引下的儿童时间认知研究范式在取得丰硕成果的同时也渐入困境。以四种常见范式为突破口凝炼,困境是:(1)钟表时间认知范式能否涵盖儿童时间认知的全部内容;(2)各范式的使用与不同时间思维间能否有效适配;(3)共同量值系统(common magnitude system,CMS)能否从行为层面为儿童时间认知作出根源性解释;(4)在暂未发现成人时间认知特定脑机制的前提下,儿童时间认知脑机制的探索有多大的必要性与可行性等。诸困境折射出学界对儿童“经验”、“线性”时间的斜重;对“观念”、“周期性”时间的慢轻。中国时文化讲求以时间统摄空、物、人四者为一体,未来研究可将儿童时间认知归置于“时间-事件”背景之中,从具身情境、人际互动、时间言语习得与行为管控等方面超越。

关键词: 儿童时间认知, 研究范式, 时间三觉, 时间-事件

Abstract: The research on children's time cognition under the guidance of western positivism is not only fruitful but also limited to predicament. Four common research paradigms are taken as breakthrough points to conclude and the dilemmas are mainly reflected in:(1) whether the cognitive paradigms of clock time can cover all the contents of time thinking; (2) Whether the use of various paradigms can be effectively adapted to the different time thinking; (3) Whether the "common magnitude system" can provide a root explanation for children's time cognition from the behavioral perspective; (4) How necessary and feasible is it to explore the brain mechanism of children's time cognition on the premise of not discovering a specific brain mechanism of time processing in adults. These difficulties reflect the inclined weight of children's "empirical" and "linear" time made by previous researchers, so did the undervaluation of "conceptual" and "cyclical" time. Chinese "time" culture emphasizes the integration of time to capture space, objects and people. Future research can incorporate children's time cognition into the background of "time-event", and transcend the above-mentioned difficulties in terms of embodied situations, interpersonal interactions, behavior control and other aspects.

Key words: children's time cognition, research paradigms, time consciousness, time-event

中图分类号: 

  • B844
Allan, L. G. (1998). The influence of the scalar timing model on human timing research. Behavioural Processes, 44(2), 101-117.
Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions:Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD.Psychological Bulletin, 121(1), 65-94.
Basso, G., Nichelli, P., Wharton, C. M., Peterson, M., & Grafman, J. (2003). Distributed neural systems for temporal production:A functional MRI study.Brain Research Bulletin, 59(5), 405-411.
Bernat B., Moras, G., Seirul-Lo, F., & Cabot, J. (2011). Children's time production for concurrent nontemporal motor tasks. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 112(1), 151-160.
Bonn, C. D., & Cantlon, J. F. (2017). Spontaneous, modality-general abstraction of a ratio scale. Cognition, 169, 36-45.
Bonn, C. D., & Cantlon, J. F. (2012). The origins and structure of quantitative concepts. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 29(1-2), 149-173.
Bottini, R., & Casasanto, D. (2013). Space and time in the child's mind:Metaphoric or ATOMic? Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 803.https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00803
Brackbill, Y., & Fitzgerald, H. E. (1972). Stereotype temporal conditioning in infants.Psychophysiology, 9(6), 569-577.
Brannon, E. M., Libertus, M., Meck, W., & Woldorff, M. (2008). Electrophysiological measures of time processing in infant and adult brains:Weber's law holds. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(2), 193-203.
Brannon, E. M., Roussel, L. W., Meck, W., & Woldorff, M. (2004). Timing in the baby brain.Cognitive Brain Research, 21(2), 227-233.
Brannon, E. M., Suanda, S., & Libertus, K. (2007).Temporal discrimination increases in precision over development and parallels the development of numerosity discrimination. Developmental Science, 10(6), 770-777.
Bullard, S. E., Fein, D., Gleeson, M. K., Tischer, N., Mapou, R. L., & Kaplan, E. (2004). The Biber cognitive estimation test. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 19(6), 835-846.
Buriticá, J., Vilchez, Z., & Santos, C. V. D. (2016). Temporal discrimination and delayed reinforcement. Behavioural Processes, 130, 71-74.
Cainelli, E., Mioni, G., Boniver, C., Bisiacchi, P. S., & Vecchi, M. (2019). Time perception in childhood absence epilepsy:Findings from a pilot study. Epilepsy & Behavior, 99, 106460.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yebeh.2019.106460
Casasanto, D., Fotakopoulou, O., & Boroditsky, L. (2010). Space and time in the child's mind:Evidence for a cross-dimensional asymmetry.Cognitive Science, 34(3). 387-405.
Casini, L., Pech-Georgel, C., & Ziegler, J. C. (2018). It's about time:revisiting temporal processing deficits in dyslexia. Developmental Science, 21(2), e12530.https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12530
Chen, T., Liu, L. L., Cui, J. F., Chen, X. J., & Wang, Y. (2016). Developmental trajectory of time perspective:From children to older adults. PsyCh Journal, 5(4), 245-255.
Colombo, J., & Richman, W. A. (2002). Infant time keeping:attention and temporal estimation in 4-month-olds. Psychological Science, 13(5), 475-479.
Crollen, V., Grade, S., Pesenti, M., & Dormal, V. (2013). A common metric magnitude system for the perception and production of numerosity, length, and duration.Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 449.https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00449
De Hevia, M. D., Izard, V., Coubart, A., Spelke, E. S., & Streri, A. (2014). Representations of space, time, and number in neonates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111(13), 4809-4813.
Droit-volet, S. (1998). Time estimation in young children:An initial force rule governing time production. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 68(3), 236-249.
Droit-volet, S. (2013). Time perception in children:A neurodevelopmental approach. Neuropsychologia, 51(2), 220-234.
Droit-volet, S., Clément, A., & Fayol, M. (2008). Time, number and length:Similarities and differences in discrimination in adults and children. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61(12), 1827-1846.
Droit-volet, S., & Coull, J. (2015). The developmental emergence of the mental time-line:Spatial and numerical distortion of time judgement. PLoS One, 10(7), e0130465.https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0130465
Droit-volet, S., Clément, A., & Fayol, M. (2003). Time and number discrimination in a bisection task with a sequence of stimuli:A developmental approach. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 84(1), 63-76.
Droit-volet, S., & Gil, S. (2015). The emotional body and time perception. Cognition & Emotion, 30(4), 687-699.
Droit-volet, S., & Rattat, A. C. (2007). A further analysis of time bisection behavior in children with and without reference memory:The similarity and the partition task.Acta Psychologica, 125(2), 240-256.
Droit-volet, S., & Wearden, J. H. (2001). Temporal bisection in children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 80(2), 142-159.
Droit-volet, S., & Wearden, J. (2016). Passage of time judgments are not duration judgments:Evidence from a study using experience sampling methodology. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 176.https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00176
Fayolle, S. L., & Droit-volet. S. (2014). Time perception and dynamics of facial expressions of emotions. PLoS One, 9(5), e97944.https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0097944
Friedman, W. J. (1986). The development of children's knowledge of temporal structure. Child Development, 57(6), 1386-1400.
Gautier, T., & Droit-volet, S. (2002). Attention and time estimation in 5- and 8-year-old children:A dual-task procedure. Behavioural Processes, 58, 57-66.
Gil, S., Chambres, P., Hyvert, C., Fanget, M., & Droit-volet, S. (2012). Children with autism spectrum disorders have "the working raw material" for time perception. PLoS One, 7(11), e49116.https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0049116
Gil, S., Rousset, R., & Droit-volet. S. (2009). How liked and disliked foods affect time perception. Emotion, 9(4), 457-463.
Gil, S., Niedenthal, P. M., & Droit-volet, S. (2007). Anger and time perception in children. Emotion, 7(1), 219-225.
Hamamouche, K., & Cordes, S. (2019). Number, time, and space are not singularly represented:Evidence against a common magnitude system beyond early childhood. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26(3), 833-854.
Henley, T., & Hergenhahn, B. R. (2013).An Introduction to the History of Psychology(7th Ed). Cengage Learning.
Huang, J., Yang, B. R., Zou, X. B., Jing, J., Pen, G., McAlonan, G. M., & Chan, R. C. (2012). Temporal processing impairment in children with attention-deficit-hyperactivity disorder. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 33(2), 538-548.
Hurks, P. P. M., & Hendriksen, J. G. M. (2010). Retrospective and prospective time deficits in childhood ADHD:The effects of task modality, duration, and symptom dimensions. Child Neuropsychology, 17(1), 34-50.
Hviid, P. (2008). "Next year we are small, right?" Different times in children's development. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 23(2), 183-198.
Kant, I. (1990).Critique of pure reason (J. M. D. Meiklejon,Trans.). Buffalo, NY:Prometheus Books. (Original work published 1781).
Khoshnoud, S., Shamsi, M., Nazari, M. A., & Makeig, S. (2018). Different cortical source activation patterns in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder during a time reproduction task. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 40(7), 633-649.
Labrell, F., Mikaeloff, Y., Perdry, H.,& Dellatolas, G. (2016). Time knowledge acquisition in children aged 6 to 11 years and its relationship with numerical skills. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 143, 1-13.
Lewis, E. A., Zax, A., & Cordes, S. (2018). The impact of emotion on numerical estimation:a developmental perspective. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71(6), 1300-1311.
Lourenco, S., & Longo, M. (2010). General magnitude representation in human infants.Psychological Science, 21(6), 873-881.
Macar, F., Anton, J. L., Bonnet, M., & Vidal, F. (2004). Timing functions of the supplementary motor area:An event-related fMRI study. Cognitive Brain Research, 21(2), 206-215.
Mcgee, R., Brodeur, D., Symons, D., Andrade, B., & Fahie, C. (2004). Time perception:Does it distinguish ADHD and RD children in a clinical sample? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 32(5), 481-490.
McInerney, D. M. (2004). Adiscussion of future time perspective. Educational Psychology Review, 16(2), 141-151.
Michel, F., Harb, F., & Hidalgo, M. P. (2012). The concept of time in the perception of children and adolescents.Trends in Psychiatry & Psychotherapy, 34(1), 38-41.
Nazari, M. A., Mirloo, M. M., Rezaei, M., & Soltanlou, M. (2018). Emotional stimuli facilitate time perception in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Neuropsychology, 12(2), 165-175.
Neufang, S., Fink, G. R., Herpertz-Dahlmann, B., Willmes, K., & Konrad, K. (2008). Developmental changes in neural activation and psychophysiological interaction patterns of brain regions associated with interference control and time perception. NeuroImage, 43(2), 399-409.
Newcombe, N. (2014). The origins and development of magnitude estimation. Ecological Psychology, 26, 147-157.
O'Connell, B. G., & Gerard, A. B. (1985). Scripts and scraps:the development of sequential understanding. Child Development, 56(3), 671-681.
Odic, D.(2018). Children's intuitive sense of number develops independently of their perception of area, density, length, and time. Developmental Science, 21(2). e12533.https://doi.org/10.1111/desc. 12533
Odic, D., Lisboa, J. V., Eisinger, R., Olivera, M. G., Maiche, A., & Halberda, J. (2016). Approximate number and approximate time discrimination each correlate with school math abilities in young children. Acta Psychologica, 163, 17-26.
Piaget, J. (1946).Le développement de la notion de temps chezl'enfant [The child's conception of time]. Paris:PUF.
Plummer, C., & Humphrey, N. (2009). Time perception in children with ADHD:The effects of task modality and duration. Child Neuropsychology, 15(2), 147-162.
Pouthas, V., Droit, S., & Jacquet, A. Y. (1993). Temporal experiences and time knowledge in infancy and early childhood. Time & Society, 2(2), 199-218.
Provasi, J., Rattat, A., & Droit-volet, S. (2011). Temporal bisection in 4-month-old infants. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Animal Behavior Processes, 37(1), 108-113.
Quartier, V., Zimmermann, G., & Nashat, S. (2010). Sense of time in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD):A comparative study. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 69(1), 7-14.
Rubia, K., Noorloos, J., Smith, A., Gunning, B., & Sergeant, J. (2003).Motor timing deficits in community and clinical boys with hyperactive behavior the effect of methylphenidate on motor timing. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 31(3), 301-313.
Rubia, K., Smith, A., & Taylor, E. (2007). Performance of children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) on a test battery of impulsiveness.Child Neuropsychology, 13(3), 276-304.
Rubia, K., Taylor, A., Taylor, E., & Sergeant, J. A. (1999). Synchronization, anticipation, and consistency in motor timing of children with dimensionality defined attention deficit hyperactivity behaviour. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 89, 1237-1258.
Ruscher, J. B. (2012). Describinggrief under cyclical versus linear conceptions of time. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 31(3), 321-330.
Shih, L. Y. L., Kuo, W. J., Yeh, T. C., Tzeng, O. J. L., & Hsieh, J. C. (2009). Common neural mechanisms for explicit timing in the sub-second range? Neuro Report, 20(10), 897-901.
Shih, L. Y. L., Yeh, T. C., Kuo, W. J., Tzeng, O. J. L., & Hsieh, J. C. (2010). Effect of temporal difficulty on cerebrocerebellar interaction during visual duration discrimination.Behavioural Brain Research, 207(1), 155-160.
Smith, A., Taylor, E., Warner-Rogers, J., Newman, S., & Rubia, K. (2002). Evidence for a pure time perception deficit in children with ADHD.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43(4), 529-542.
Tiffin-Richards, M. C., Hasselhorn, M., Richards, M. L., Banaschewski, T., & Rothenberger, A. (2004). Time reproduction in finger tapping tasks by children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and/or dyslexia. Dyslexia, 10(4), 299-315.
Tillman, K. A., Marghetis, T., Barner, D.,& Srinivasan, M. (2017). Today is tomorrow's yesterday:Children's acquisition of deictic time words. Cognitive Psychology, 92, 87-100.
Tillman, K. A., Tulagan, N., Fukuda, E., & Barner, D. (2018). The mental timeline is gradually constructed in childhood. Developmental Science, 21(6), e12679.https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12679
Tobia, V., Rinaldi, L., & Marzocchi, G. M. (2018). Time processing impairments in preschoolers at risk of developing difficulties in mathematics. Developmental Science, 21(2), e12526.https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12526
Toplak, M. E., Rucklidge, J. J., Hetherington, R., John, S. C. F., & Tannock, R. (2003). Time perception deficits in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and comorbid reading difficulties in child and adolescent samples.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44(6), 888-903.
Toplak, M. E., & Tannock, R. (2005). Time perception:Modality and duration effects in attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder.Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 33(5), 639-654.
Twenge, J. M., Catanese, K. R., & Baumeister, R. F. (2003). Social exclusion and the deconstructed state:Time perception, meaninglessness, lethargy, lack of emotion, and self-awareness. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 85(3), 409-423.
VanMarle, K., & Wynn, K. (2006). Six-month-olds infants use analog magnitudes to represent durations.Developmental Science, 9(5), F41-F49.
Walsh, V. (2003). A theory of magnitude:common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity.Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(11), 483-488.
Wiener, M., Turkeltaub, P., & Coslett, H. B. (2010). The image of time:A voxel-wise meta-analysis.NeuroImage, 49(2), 1728-1740.
Wilkening, F. (1981). Integrating velocity, time, and distance information:A developmental study. Cognitive Psychology, 13(2), 231-247.
Witowska, J., Zajenkowski, M., & Wittmann, M. (2020). Integration of balanced time perspective and time perception:The role of executive control and neuroticism. Personality and Individual Differences, 163, 110061.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2020.110061
Wittmann, M., Rudolph, T., Linares Gutierrez, D., & Winkler, I. (2015). Timeperspective and emotion regulation as predictors of age-related subjective passage of time. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 12(12), 16027-16042.
Zakay, D. (1990).The evasive art of subjective time measurement:Some methodological dilemmas. In R. A. Block (Ed.), Cognitive models of psychological time(pp.59-84). Hillsdale, NJ:Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Zakay, D., & Block, R. A. (1996). The role of attention in time estimation processes. Advances in Psychology, 115, 143-164.
Zakay, D., & Fleisig, D. (2011). Psychological time, time perspective, culture and conflict resolution. In S. Han & E. P ppel (Eds.), Culture and Neural Frames of Cognition and Communication(pp.123-137). Springer-Verlag, Berlin, Heidelberg.
埃斯, B. G. (2017). 孩子与时间(林晓轩译). 北京:生活·读书·新知三联书店.
方格. (1987). 儿童对时间认知的发展. 心理科学进展, (4), 21-26.
方格, 冯刚, 姜涛, 方富熹. (1993). 学前儿童对时距的估计及其策略. 心理学报, 25(4), 12-18.
黄希庭, 杨宗义, 刘中华. (1980). 5至9岁儿童时间观念发展的实验研究. 西南师范学院学报(自然科学版), (1), 67-76.
黄希庭, 张增杰. (1979). 5至8岁儿童时间知觉的实验研究. 心理学报, 11(2), 26-34.
林泳海. (1996). 4.5~7.5岁儿童时间持续认知发展的实验研究. 心理发展与教育, 12(1), 17-21.
苏佳佳, 叶浩生. (2020). 中国神话中的具身心理学思想探索.心理学报, 52(3), 386-398.
陶云, 马谐. (2010). 面孔情绪下3-8岁儿童时距知觉的实验研究. 心理发展与教育, 26(3), 225-232.
吴睿明, 黄彦萍, 李红, 冯廷勇, 龙长权. (2005). 3.5~5.5岁儿童时序认知能力的发展研究. 心理发展与教育, 21(3), 7-12.
王小英, 杨海燕, 王丽娟, 石友梅. (2006). 4~6岁儿童未来时间认知教学的实验研究. 心理科学, 29(5), 1063-1067+1048.
夏青. (2018). 技术时代下儿童时间的教育阐释. 中国教育学刊, (4), 52-56.
余习德, 李明, 夏新懿, 朱一奕, 高定国. (2019). 儿童时间言语的发生与发展. 学前教育研究, (2), 57-69.
姚竹曦, 张亮, 张侃. (2015). 标量计时模型的影响因素及发展. 心理科学进展, 23(5), 784-792.
朱曼殊, 宋正国, C.B.哈维. (1987). 中国和加拿大儿童对持续时间概念的掌握. 心理科学, (2), 1-4 + 17 + 64.
周榕, 黄希庭. (2001). 儿童时间隐喻表征能力的发展研究. 心理科学, 24(5), 606-607+609.
[1] 林崇德. 需要是教育与发展的动力源[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(1): 1-7.
[2] 冷欣怡, 苏萌萌, 李文玲, 杨秀杰, 邢爱玲, 张湘琳, 舒华. 家庭环境与农村儿童早期语言发展的关系[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(1): 8-18.
[3] 陈书玲, 贾会宾, 靳璨, 张欣, 王恩国. 发展性协调障碍儿童视觉通道前注意加工的特点:来自ERP的证据[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(1): 19-28.
[4] 谢和平, 王燕青, 王福兴, 周宗奎, 邓素娥, 段朝辉. 记忆的生成绘图效应及其边界条件:一项元分析[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(1): 29-43.
[5] 田惠东, 张玉红, 孙昊翔, 李瑛, 哈尼夏. 师生关系对智力障碍学生同伴交往能力的影响:有调节的中介模型[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(1): 44-54.
[6] 陈启山, 何静怡. 女大学生室友的愤怒特质匹配与人际关系:有调节的多项式回归模型[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(1): 55-63.
[7] 王鑫强, 李金文, 卢红燕, 赖正伟, 李佳源. 择偶优劣势对师范大学生教师职业认同的影响:择偶价值感的中介及自尊的调节作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(1): 64-73.
[8] 赵京伟, 陈晓旭, 任立文, 耿喆, 徐夫真. 父母心理控制与小学儿童焦虑:一个有调节的中介模型[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(1): 93-102.
[9] 秦瑶, 彭运石. 父母教养方式对初中生社交焦虑的影响:同伴接纳和反刍思维的链式中介作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(1): 103-113.
[10] 魏华, 丁慧敏, 陈武, 郝兴风, 熊婕. 父母低头行为与青少年网络欺负的关系:压力的中介作用与年龄的调节作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(1): 114-121.
[11] 樊香麟, 崔英锦. 客体化身体意识与女大学生限制性饮食行为的关系:外貌负面评价恐惧和社会文化压力的中介作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(1): 122-131.
[12] 张慧如, 张伟达, 傅王倩, 邓敏, 彭苏浩, 李玉. 孤独感对创造性倾向的影响:无聊倾向和焦虑情绪的中介作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(1): 132-141.
[13] 王伊萌, 王振东, 汪凤炎. “当局者迷”破解之道:智慧的正念干预研究——个人成长主动性与反思的作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(1): 142-152.
[14] 金星, 刘景弘, 马跃, 于战宇. 聋童和正常儿童身体表情注意瞬脱的比较研究[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(3): 305-312.
[15] 钟伟芳, 郭永兴. 语言范畴影响面部表情知觉的电生理学证据[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(3): 313-322.
Viewed
Full text


Abstract

Cited

  Shared   
  Discussed   
No Suggested Reading articles found!