心理发展与教育 ›› 2022, Vol. 38 ›› Issue (2): 161-170.doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2022.02.02

• 认知与社会性发展 • 上一篇    下一篇

记忆编码策略对心境一致性记忆的调节作用

马小凤1,2, 文美琪1,2, 史凯1,2, 杜青青1,2, 姚贝1,2, 穆缘1,2, 霍小宁3   

  1. 1. 西北师范大学心理学院, 兰州 730030;
    2. 甘肃省行为与心理重点实验室, 兰州 730030;
    3. 兰州市第三人民医院, 兰州 730070
  • 发布日期:2022-03-24
  • 通讯作者: 霍小宁, 文美琪 E-mail:lzsyhxn@163.com;meigi.wen@foxmail.com
  • 基金资助:
    国家自然科学基金地区项目(31860282);兰州市人才创新创业项目(2017-RC-7);兰州市卫生健康科技发展项目(2021003)。

“Retrieve Practice” Impede Mood Congruence in Memory

MA Xiaofeng1,2, WEN Meiqi1,2, SHI Kai1,2, DU Qingqing1,2, YAO Bei1,2, MU Yuan1,2, HUO Xiaoning3   

  1. 1. School of Psychology, Northwest Normal University, Lanzhou 730030;
    2. Key Laboratory of Behavioral and Mental Health of Gansu Province, Lanzhou 730030;
    3. The Third People's Hospital of Lanzhou, Lanzhou 730070
  • Published:2022-03-24

摘要: 情绪对记忆的影响会受记忆编码策略的调节。本研究通过2个实验探讨编码策略对心境一致性记忆的调节作用。实验1和实验2均采用2(编码策略:提取练习策略、重复学习策略)×2(词性:消极词、中性词)的实验设计,分别探讨了不同记忆编码策略对普通大学生(实验1)和非临床抑郁大学生(实验2)心境一致性记忆的影响。结果发现:无论是被成功诱导出抑郁情绪的普通大学生,还是非临床抑郁倾向大学生,使用重复学习策略时回忆出的消极词均显著多于中性词,表现出显著的心境一致性记忆,而使用提取练习策略时回忆出的消极词和中性词没有显著性差异,未表现出心境一致性记忆。实验结果表明,记忆编码策略对心境一致性记忆具有调节作用,提取练习策略能够抑制心境一致性记忆。研究结果指明了心境一致性记忆的边界条件,并对非临床抑郁个体的学习与记忆具有重要的启示。

关键词: 提取练习, 心境一致性效应, 情绪, 抑郁倾向, 情绪渗透模型

Abstract: A wealth of researches has indicated that emotions can affect different memory processing stages, such as memory coding, storage and retrieval. However, the impact of emotions on memory coding will vary depending on the coding strategy. The affect infusion model believes that the generation of mood-consistent memory to a large extent depends on the processing strategies used by individuals in specific situations. When adopting high-permeability processing strategies(such as heuristic strategies:tasks are relatively simple, familiar, and self-independent), the degree of emotional penetration is higher; when using low-permeability processing strategies(such as motivation-driven strategies:have clear goals and motivations, and occupy a large number of cognitive resources), the degree of emotional penetration is lower. Current research on mood-consistent memory principally focuses on the effects of explicit and implicit processing methods on mood-consistent memory. No studies have yet explored the inhibitory effect of low-permeability processing strategies on mood-consistent memory at the level of explicit memory. In the study, two experiments were conducted to test the effect of coding strategies on mood-consistent memory. In Experiment 1, adopting ordinary college students as subjects, and the experimental design of 2 (coding strategy:retrieve practice strategy, repetitive learning strategy)×2 (part of speech:negative words, neutral words). The purpose of Experiment 1 was to explore the effects of different memory coding strategies on mood-consistent memory. It was found that the main effect of coding strategy is significant, the main effect of part of speech is significant, and the interaction between coding strategy and part of speech is significant. A further simple effect analysis found that the subjects recalled more negative words than neutral words when using the repetitive learning strategy, showing obvious mood-consistent memory. When using retrieve practice strategy, no matter whether it was in the final test or in the three extractions during the learning phase, the recalled negative words and neutral words had no significant difference, which effectively suppressed the mood-consistent memory. In Experiment 2, adopting non-clinical depression college students as subjects, and also adopted the experimental design of 2 (coding strategy:retrieve practice strategy, repetitive learning strategy)×2 (part of speech:negative words, neutral words), which was aimed to study the effect of different memory coding strategies on mood-consistent memory. The results were the same as in Experiment 1.The experiment had the following results. Firstly, memory coding strategy has a regulating effect on mood-consistent memory, and retrieve practice strategy as a high-quality coding strategy can effectively suppress mood-consistent memory. Secondly, the research results indicate the boundary conditions of mood-consistent memory and have important implications for the learning and memory of non-clinical depressed individuals.

Key words: retrieve practice, mood-consistent effects, emotions, depression tendency, affect infusion model

中图分类号: 

  • B844
Beck, A. T. (2008). The evolution of the cognitive model of depression and its neurobiological correlates.American Journal of Psychiatry, 165(8), 969-977.
Bjork, R. A. (1975).Retrieval as a memory modifier. In R. Solso (Ed.), Information processing and cognition:The Loyola Symposium (pp. 123-144). Hillsdale, NJ:Erlbaum.
Blaney, P. H. (1986). Affect and memory:A review. Psychological Bulletin, 99(2), 229-246.
Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and Memory.American Psychologist, 36(2), 129-148.
Bower, G. H. (1987). Commentary on mood and memory.Behaviour Research and Therapy, 25(6), 443-455.
Bradley, B. P., Mogg, K., & Williams, R. (1994). Implicit and explicit memory for emotional information in non-clinical subjects.Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32(1), 65-78.
Brewer, D. H., Doughtie, E. B., & Lubin, B. (1980). Induction of Mood and Mood Shift.Journal of Clinical Psychology, 36(1), 215-226.
Buchanan, T. W. (2007). Retrieval of emotional memories.Psychological Bulletin, 133(5), 761-779.
Carpenter, S. K. (2011). Semantic information activated during retrieval contributes to later retention:Support for the mediator effectiveness hypothesis of the testing effect.Journal of Experimental Psychology:Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37(6), 1547-1552.
Cook, E. W., Atkinson, L. S., & Lang, K. G. (1987). Stimulus control and data acquisition for IBM PCs and compatibles.Psychophysiology,24(6), 726-727
Deveney, C. M., & Deldin, P. J. (2004). Memory of faces:A slow wave ERP study of major depression. Emotion, 4(3), 295-304.
Dolcos, F., Labar, K. S., & Cabeza, R. (2005). Remembering one year later:Role of the amygdala and the medial temporal lobe memory system in retrieving emotional memories.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102(7), 2626-2631.
Eich, E., & Forgas, J. P. (2003). Mood, cognition, and memory. In A. F. Healy, & R. W. Proctor (Eds.), Handbook of psychology:Experimental psychology (Vol. 4, pp. 61-83). New York:Wiley.
Gaddy, M. A., & Ingram, R. E. (2014). A meta-analytic review of mood-congruent implicit memory in depressed mood. Clinical Psychology Review, 34(5), 402-416.
Hadley,C. B., & MacKay,D. G. (2006). Does emotion help or hinder immediate memory? Arousal versus priority-binding mechanisms. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Learning,Memory,and Cognition,32,79- 88.
Hamann, S. (2001). Cognitive and neural mechanisms of emotional memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5(9), 394-400.
Joormann, J., Teachman, B. A., & Gotlib, I. H. (2009). Sadder and less accurate? False memory for negative material in depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118(2), 412-417.
Karpicke, J. D., Butler, A. C., & Roediger III, H. L. (2009).Metacognitive strategies in student learning:Do students practise retrieval when they study on their own? Memory, 17(4), 471-479.
Karpicke, J. D., & Blunt, J. R. (2011). Retrieval practice produces more learning than elaborative studying with concept mapping. Science, 331(6018), 772-775.
Karpicke, J. D., Lehman, M., & Aue, W. R. (2014). Retrieval-based learning:An episodic context account.In B. H. Ross(Ed.). The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 61, pp. 237-284). Academic Press.
Karpicke, J. D., & Roediger, H. L. (2008). The critical importance of retrieval for learning. Science, 319(5865), 966-968.
Kensinger, E. A., & Corkin, S. (2004). Two routes to emotional memory:Distinct neural processes for valence and arousal.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 101(9), 3310-3315.
Knott, L. M., & Thorley, C. (2014). Mood congruent false memories persist over time.Cognition & Emotion, 28(5), 903-912.
Lehman, M., Smith, M. A., & Karpicke, J. D. (2014). Toward an episodic context account of retrieval-based learning:Dissociating retrieval practice and elaboration. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Learning, Memory and Cognition, 40(6), 1787-1794.
Lewinsohn, P. M., & Rosenbaum, M. (1987). Recall of parental behavior by acute depressives, remitted depressives, and nondepressives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(3), 611-619.
Maria, G., & Juan, G. (2017). Negative bias in the perception and memory of emotional information in alzheimer disease.Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology, 30(3), 131-139.
Martin, M. (1990). On the induction of mood.Clinical Psychology Review, 10(6), 669-697.
Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (2005). Cognitive vulnerability to emotional disorders.Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 1, 167-195.
Mayer, J. D., Allen, J. P., & Beauregard, K. (1995). Mood inductions for four specific moods:A procedure employing guided imagery vignettes with music.Journal of Mental Imagery, 19(1-2), 151-159.
McGaugh, J. L., & Roozendaal, B. (2002). Role ofadrenal stress hormones in forming lasting memories in the brain. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 12(2), 205-210.
Phelps, E. A. (2004). Human emotion and memory:Interactions of the amygdala and hippocampal complex. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 14(2), 198-202.
Provencio, M., Vazquez, C., Valiente, C., & Hervas, G. (2012). Depressive primes stimulate initial avoidance of angry faces:An eye-tracking study of paranoid ideation.Cognitive Therapy and Research, 36(5), 483-492.
Pyc, M. A., & Rawson, K. A. (2009). Testing the retrieval effort hypothesis:Does greater difficulty correctly recalling information lead to higher levels of memory? Journal of Memory and Language, 60(4), 437-447.
Roediger, H. L., & Karpicke, J. D. (2006). Test-enhanced learning taking memory tests improves long-term retention. Psychological Science, 17(3), 249-255.
Rowland, C. A., Littrell-Baez, M. K., Sensenig, A. E., & DeLosh, E. L. (2014). Testing effects in mixed-versus pure-list designs.Memory & Cognition, 42(6), 912-921.
Ruci, L., Tomes, J. L., & Zelenski, J. M. (2009). Mood-congruent false memories in the DRM paradigm.Cognition & Emotion, 23(6), 1153-1165.
Scher, C. D., Ingram, R. E., & Segal, Z. V. (2005). Cognitive reactivity and vulnerability:Empirical evaluation of construct activation and cognitive diatheses in unipolar depression. Clinical Psychology Review, 25(4), 487-510.
Singer, J. A., & Salovey, P. (1988). Mood and memory:Evaluating the Network Theory of Affect. Clinical Psychology Review, 8(2), 211-251.
Smith, A. M., Floerke, V. A., & Thomas, A. K. (2016). Retrieval practice protects memory against acute stress.Science, 354(6315), 1046-1048.
Smith, A. M., & Thomas, A. K. (2018). Reducing the Consequences of Acute Stress on Memory Retrieval. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 7(2), 219-229.
Stenson, A. F., Leventon, J. S., & Bauer, P. J. (2019). Emotion effects on memory from childhood through adulthood:Consistent enhancement and adult gender differences. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,178, 121-136.
Taylor, J. L., & John, C. H. (2004). Attentional and memory bias in persecutory delusions and depression.Psychopathology, 37(5), 233-241.
Watkins, P. C., Mathews, A., Williamson, D. A., & Fuller, R. D. (1992). Mood-congruent memory in depression:Emotional priming or elaboration? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101(3), 581-586.
Westermann, R., Spies, K., Stahl, G., & Hesse, F. W. (1996). Relative effectiveness and validity of mood induction procedures:a meta-analysis. European Journal of Social Psychology, 26(4), 557-580.
Wing, E. A., Marsh, E. J., & Cabeza, R. (2013). Neural correlates of retrieval-based memory enhancement:An fMRI study of thetesting effect. Neuropsychologia, 51(12), 2360-2370.
Wisco, B. E. (2009). Depressive cognition:Self-reference and depth of processing.Clinical Psychology Review, 29(4), 382-392.
Zhang, W., Gross, J., & Hayne, H. (2017). The effect of mood on false memory for emotional DRM word lists. Cognition & Emotion, 31(3), 526-537.
丁道群, 郭程, 尹霖, 来棘.(2018).不同注意资源条件下厌恶与恐惧情绪的记忆增强效应. 中国临床心理学杂志, 26(05), 835-840.
郭力平. (1997). 再认记忆测验中抑郁个体的心境一致性记忆研究. 心理学报, 29(4), 357-363.
骆宏, 马剑虹. (2004). 大学生抑郁症状、问题解决能力与压力感知的关系分析. 中国临床心理学杂志, 12(04), 367-368+364.
毛新瑞, 徐慧芳, 郭春彦. (2015). 双加工再认提取中的情绪记忆增强效应. 心理学报, 47(9), 1111-1123.
屈智勇, 张秀兰. (2008). 中西部大学生抑郁现状、人群特征及干预效果. 清华大学学报:哲学社会科学版, 23(2), 120-134.
谭曦, 张靖, 吴朝阳, 杜渐, 孔军辉. (2010). 大学生抑郁倾向的特点及中医心理干预. 吉林中医药, 30(9), 741-744.
王海宝, 张达人, 余永强. (2009). 情绪记忆增强效应的时间依赖性. 心理学报, 41(10), 932-938.
谢韵梓, 阳泽. (2016). 不同情绪诱发方法有效性的比较研究.心理与行为研究, 14(5), 591-599.
[1] 梁丹丹, 闫晓民, 葛志林. 4~8岁汉语高功能自闭症儿童基于语言线索的情绪识别能力发展研究[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(2): 169-175.
[2] 韩建涛, 钱俊妮, 张婕妤, 庞维国. 创造力与大学生生命意义感:积极情绪和创造性自我效能感的作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(2): 187-195.
[3] 刘岩, 张宏飞, 卢鑫, 曲可佳. 数学概率问题解决学习中的测试效应及有效促进[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(2): 215-223.
[4] 洪新伟, 苗灵童, 范航, 宋明华, 朱婷婷, 刘燊, 张林. 父母婚姻冲突与青少年攻击行为的关系:情绪安全感和学校联结的作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(5): 726-734.
[5] 李甜甜, 董会芹. 父母冲突知觉与儿童焦虑情绪:正负性信息注意偏向的中介作用及性别的调节作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(4): 488-496.
[6] 金星, 刘景弘, 马跃, 于战宇. 聋童和正常儿童身体表情注意瞬脱的比较研究[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(3): 305-312.
[7] 张鹏程, 李喜, 韩午阳, 沈永江. 睡眠不足对中小学生负性情绪的影响:一个链式中介模型[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(3): 402-409.
[8] 李其容, 杨艳宇, 李春萱. 父母冲突、青少年日常自我效能和学习投入:日常消极情绪与日常沉思在父母冲突调节效应中的中介作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(2): 236-246.
[9] 鲁明辉, 王融, 张丽敏. 父母心理弹性与自闭症谱系障碍儿童情绪行为问题的关系:一个有调节的中介模型[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(2): 247-254.
[10] 李奕萱, 王泽宇, 周铭扬, 杨玉川, 赵敏翔, 董妍. 高中生物理和化学学科中同伴学业支持与学业情绪的关系:学习效能感的中介作用和性别的调节作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(1): 58-67.
[11] 蒋索, 丁金琦, 刘艳, 陆元圆, 李小青, 陈静. 青少年早期网络欺凌/受欺凌对睡眠质量的影响:社交焦虑和抑郁情绪的链式中介作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(1): 85-96.
[12] 汪玥, 张豹, 周晖. 中小学生正念注意觉知与心理健康:情绪调节和积极重评的跨时间中介作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2022, 38(5): 692-702.
[13] 黄潇潇, 张宝山, 俞国良. 日常小团体多样性与老年人身体症状的关系:团体积极情绪的中介作用和心理压力感的调节作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2022, 38(5): 729-738.
[14] 郎悦茹, 龚少英, 曹阳, 吴亚男. 网络学习中师生交互与大学生学习投入的关系:自主动机与学业情绪的序列中介作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2022, 38(4): 530-537.
[15] 杨晨, 连帅磊, 陈春宇, 孙晓军, 周宗奎. 同伴依恋与初中生手机成瘾的关系:有调节的中介效应分析[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2022, 38(4): 538-545.
Viewed
Full text


Abstract

Cited

  Shared   
  Discussed   
No Suggested Reading articles found!