心理发展与教育 ›› 2017, Vol. 33 ›› Issue (1): 113-121.doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2017.01.13

• 理论与方法 • 上一篇    下一篇

发展性阅读障碍亚类型研究进展

孟泽龙1,2, 张逸玮3, 毕鸿燕1,2   

  1. 1. 中国科学院行为科学重点实验室, 中国科学院心理研究所, 北京 100101;
    2. 中国科学院大学, 北京 100049;
    3. 中国人民大学劳动人事学院, 北京 100872
  • 出版日期:2017-01-15 发布日期:2017-01-15
  • 通讯作者: 毕鸿燕,E-mail:bihy@psych.ac.cn E-mail:bihy@psych.ac.cn
  • 基金资助:

    本项目得到国家自然科学基金面上项目(31371044,31671155)资助研究。

Advances in Research on Subtypes of Developmental Dyslexia

MENG Zelong1,2, ZHANG Yiwei3, BI Hongyan1,2   

  1. 1. CAS Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology, Beijing 100101;
    2. University of Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100049;
    3. School of Labor and Human Resources, Renmin University of China, Beijing 100872
  • Online:2017-01-15 Published:2017-01-15

摘要: 发展性阅读障碍是一种特殊的学习障碍,伴有多种认知缺陷并且存在不同的亚类型。依据相关的阅读模型理论,阅读障碍可划分为语音型和表层型。从认知缺陷角度出发,语音加工缺陷是主要的缺陷表现,以此为特征形成一种主要的阅读障碍的亚类型,同时还有以正字法加工缺陷和快速命名缺陷为主的其他亚类型。而以基本感知觉缺陷为标准,主要有以视觉加工缺陷和以听觉加工缺陷为主的两种亚类型。在汉语条件下,依据同样的阅读模型理论,语音型阅读障碍亚类型比例明显低于拼音文字条件下的。汉语阅读障碍也具有分别以语音加工缺陷、快速命名缺陷和正字法加工缺陷为主要认知缺陷的亚类型。未来有必要从神经机制角度进一步明确不同亚类型的神经基础。

关键词: 发展性阅读障碍, 亚类型, 双通路模型, 语音缺陷

Abstract: Developmental dyslexia is a kind of learning disability and has various subtypes. According to reading model, children with dyslexia are classified to phonological dyslexia and surface dyslexia in alphabetic languages. From the perspective of cognitive deficits, there are phonological processing deficit, orthographic processing deficit and rapid naming deficit. In the three deficits, phonological processing deficit is a main subtype that dyslexics show difficulty in phonological processing. Based on the basic sensory-perceptual processing deficit, some dyslexia can be devided into visual processing deficit subtype and auditory processing deficit subtype. However, the ratio of phonological subtype, when classified according to the same reading model in Chinese children with dyslexia, is much lower than that in alphabetic language. Chinese dyslexic children can also be classified to three subtypes including phonological processing deficit, orthographic processing deficit and rapid naming deficit in the light of cognitive deficits. Further studies are highly needed to clarify the neural mechanism of different subtypes in developmental dyslexia.

Key words: developmental dyslexia, subtypes, dual-route model, phonological deficit

中图分类号: 

  • B844

Anderson, R. C., Li, W., Ku, Y-M., Shu, H., & Wu, N. (2003). Use of partial information in learning to read Chinese characters. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(1), 52-57.

Araújo S., Pacheco A., Faísca L., Petersson K. M., & Reis A. (2010). Visual rapid naming and phonological abilities:Different subtypes in dyslexic children. International Journal of Psychology, 45(6), 443-452.

Badian, N. A. (1997). Dyslexia and the double deficit hypothesis. Annals of Dyslexia, 47(1), 69-87.

Badian, N. A. (2005). Does a visual-orthographic deficit contribute to reading disability? Ann Dyslexia, 55(1), 28-52.

Baldeweg, T., Richardson, A., Watkins, S., Foale, C., & Gruzelier, J. (1999). Impaired auditory frequency discrimination in dyslexia detected with mismatch evoked potentials. Annals of Neurology, 45(4), 495-503.

Baron, J., & Strawson, C. (1976). Use of orthographic and word-specific knowledge in reading words aloud. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Human Perception and Performance, 2(3), 386-393.

Béland, R., & Mimounia, Z. (2001). Deep dyslexia in the two languages of an Arabic/French bilingual patient. Cognition, 82(2), 77-126.

Benassi, M., Simonelli, L., Giovagnoli, S., & Bolzani, R. (2010). Coherence motion perception in developmental dyslexia:a meta-analysis of behavioral studies. Dyslexia, 16(4), 341-357.

Boets, B., Wouters, J., van Wieringen, A., & Ghesquièrea, P. (2006). Coherent motion detection in preschool children at family risk for dyslexia. Vision Research, 46(4), 527-535.

Booth, J. R., Lu, D., Burman, D. D., Chou, T. L., Jin, Z., Peng, D. L., et al. (2006). Specialization of phonological and semantic processing in Chinese word reading. Brain Research, 1071(1), 197-207.

Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. E. (1978). Difficulties in auditory organisation as a possible cause of reading backwardness. Nature, 271(5647), 746-747.

Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. E. (1983). Categorizing sounds and learning to read-a causal connection. Nature, 301(5899), 419-421.

Castles, A., Bates, T. C., & Coltheart, M. (2006). John Marshall and the developmental dyslexias. Aphasiology, 20(9-11), 871-892.

Castles, A., & Coltheart, M. (1993). Varieties of developmental dyslexia. Cognition, 47(1), 149-180

Catts. (1986). Speech production/phonological deficits in reading-disordered children. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 19(8), 504-508.

Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC:A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108(1), 204-256.

Corcos, E., & Willows, D. (1993). The role of visual processing in good and poor readers' utilization of orthographic information in letter strings. In S. F. Wright & R. Groner (Eds.), Facets of dyslexia and its remediation (pp.95-106). Amsterdam, Netherlands:North-Holland/Elsevier Science Publishers.

Denckla, M. B., & Rudel, R. (1974). Rapid "automatized" naming of pictured objects, colors, letters and numbers by normal children. Cortex:A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 10(1), 186-202.

Di Filippo, G., Zoccolotti, P., & Ziegler, J. C. (2008). Rapid naming deficits in dyslexia:A stumbling block for the perceptual anchor theory of dyslexia. Developmental Science, 11(6), 40-47.

Eden, G. F., Stein, J. F., Wood, H. M., & Wood, F. B. (1995). Temporal and spatial processing in reading disabled and normal children. Cortex:A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 31(3), 451-468.

Furnes, B., & Samuelsson, S. (2011). Phonological awareness and rapid automatized naming predicting early development in reading and spelling:Results from a cross-linguistic longitudinal study. Learning and Individual Differences, 21(1), 85-95.

Georgiou, G. K., Papadopoulos, T. C., Zarouna, E., & Parrila, R. (2012). Are auditory and visual processing deficits related to developmental dyslexia? Dyslexia, 18(2), 110-129.

Goswami, U., Thomson, J., Richardson, U., Stainthorp, R., Hughes, D., Rosen, S., & Scott, S. K. (2002). Amplitude envelope onsets and developmental dyslexia:A new hypothesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 99(16), 10911-10916.

Harm, M. W., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1999). Phonology, reading acquisition, and dyslexia:insights from connectionist models. Psychological Review, 106(3), 491-528.

Heath, S. M., Hogben, J. H., & Clark, C. D. (1999). Auditory temporal processing in disabled readers with and without oral language delay. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 40(4), 637-647.

Heim S., Tschierse J., Amunts K., Wilms M., Vossel S., Willmes K., Grabowska A. & Huber W. (2008). Cognitive subtypes of dyslexia. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis, 68(1),73-82.

Ho, C. S.H., & Bryant, P. (1997). Phonological skills are important in learning to read Chinese. Developmental Psychology, 33(1), 946-951.

Ho, C. S.H., Chan, D. W., Chung, K. K. H., Lee, S.-H., & Tsang, S.M. (2007). In search of subtypes of Chinese developmental dyslexia. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 97(1), 61-83.

Ho, C. S.H., Chan, D. W.O., Lee, S.H., Tsang, S.M., & Luana, V. H. (2004). Cognitive profiling and preliminary subtyping in Chinese developmental dyslexia. Cognition, 91(1), 43-75.

Ho, C. S.H., Chan, D. W.-O., Tsang, S.M., & Lee, S.H. (2002). The cognitive profile and multiple-deficit hypothesis in Chinese developmental dyslexia. Developmental Psychology, 38(4), 543-553.

Hoeft, F., Meyler, A., Hernandez, A., Juel, C., Taylor-Hill, H., Martindale, J. L.,et al. (2007). Functional and morphometric brain dissociation between dyslexia and reading ability. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(10), 4234-4239.

Jednoróg, K., Gawron, N., Marchewka, A., Heim, S., & Grabowska, A. (2014). Cognitive subtypes of dyslexia are characterized by distinct patterns of grey matter volume. Brain Structure & Function, 219(5), 1697-1707.

Kevan, A., & Pammer, K. (2008). Visual deficits in pre-readers at familial risk for dyslexia. Vision Research, 48(28), 2835-2839.

Kevan, A., & Pammer, K. (2009). Predicting early reading skills from pre-reading measures of dorsal stream functioning. Neuropsychologia, 47(14), 3174-3181.

Lawton, T. (2011). Improving magnocellular function in the dorsal stream remediates reading deficits. Optometry & Vision Development, 42(3), 142-154.

Lei, L., Pan, J., Liu, H., McBride-Chang, C., Li, H., Zhang, Y., et al. (2011). Developmental trajectories of reading development and impairment from ages 3 to 8 years in Chinese children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52(2), 212-220.

Liao, C.H., Deng, C., Hamilton, J., Lee, C. S.C., Wei, W., & Georgiou, G. K. (2015). The role of rapid naming in reading development and dyslexia in Chinese. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 130(1), 106-122.

Liao, C. H., Georgiou, G. K., & Parrila, R. (2008). Rapid naming speed and Chinese character recognition. Reading and Writing, 21(3), 231-253.

Liu, L., Peng, D., Ding, G., Jin, Z., Zhang, L., Li, K., & Chen, C. (2006). Dissociation in the neural basis underlying Chinese tone and vowel production. NeuroImage, 29(2), 515-523.

Livingstone, M. S., Rosen, G. D., Drislane, F. W., & Galaburda, A. M. (1991). Physiological and anatomical evidence for a magnocellular defect in developmental dyslexia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 88(1), 7943-7947.

Lovegrove, W. J., Bowling, A., Badcock, D., & Blackwood, M. (1980). Specific reading disability:Differences in contrast sensitivity as a function of spatial frequency. Science, 210(1), 439-479.

Lyon, G. R., Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (2003). A definition of dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 53(1), 1-14.

Manis, F. R., Seidenberg, M. S., Doi, L. M., McBride-Chang, C., & Petersen, A. (1996). On the bases of two subtypes of developmental dyslexia. Cognition, 58(2), 157-195.

Mann, V. A., & Liberman, I. Y. (1984). Phonological awareness and verbal short-term memory. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 17(10), 592-599.

McLean, G. M. T., Stuart, G. W., Coltheart, V., & Castles, A. (2011). Visual temporal processing in dyslexia and the magnocellular deficit theory:The need for speed? Journal of Experimental Psychology:Human Perception and Performance, 37(6), 1957-1975.

Meyer, M. S., Wood, B. F., Hart, L. & Felton, H. R. (1998). Longitudinal Course of Rapid Naming in Disabled and Nondisabled Readers. Annals of Dyslexia, 48(1), 91-114.

Morris, R. D., Stuebing, K. K., Fletcher, J. M., Shaywitz, S. E., Lyon, G. R., Shankweiler, D. P., et al. (1998). Subtypes of reading disability:Variability around a phonological core. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90(3), 347-373.

Nagarajan, S., Mahncke, H., Salz, T., Tallal, P., Roberts, T., & Merzenich, M. M. (1999). Cortical auditory signal processing in poor readers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 96(11), 6483-6488.

Nicolson, R. I., Fawcett, A. J., & Dean, P. (2001). Developmental dyslexia:the cerebellar deficit hypothesis. Trends in Neurosciences, 24(9), 508-511.

Norton, E. S., & Wolf, M. (2012). Rapid automatized naming (RAN) and reading fluency:Implications for understanding and treatment of reading disabilities. Annual Review of Psychology, 63(1), 427-452.

O'Brien, B., Wolf, M., Miller, L., Lovett, M., & Morris, R. (2011). Orthographic processing efficiency in developmental dyslexia:an investigation of age and treatment factors at the sublexical level. Annals of Dyslexia, 61(1), 111-135.

Peterson, R. L., Pennington, B. F., & Olson, R. K. (2013). Subtypes of developmental dyslexia:Testing the predictions of the dual-route and connectionist frameworks. Cognition, 126(1), 20-38.

Perfetti, C. A., Liu, Y., & Tan, L. H. (2005). The lexical constituency model:Some implications of research on chinese for general theories of reading. Psychological Review, 112(1), 43-59.

Qian, Y., & Bi, H. Y. (2015).The visual magnocellular deficit in Chinese-speaking children with developmental dyslexia. Frontiers in Psychology, 5(12),692-701

Ramus, F. (2003). Developmental dyslexia:specific phonological deficit or general sensorimotor dysfunction? Current opinion in neurobiology, 13(2), 212-218.

Ramus, F., Rosen, S., Dakin, S. C., Day, B. L., Castellote, J. M., White, S., & Frith, U. (2003). Theories of developmental dyslexia:insights from a multiple case study of dyslexic adults. Brain, 126(4), 841-865.

Shu, H., McBride-Chang, C., Wu, S., & Liu, H. (2006). Understanding Chinese developmental dyslexia:Morphological awareness as a core cognitive construct. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98(1), 122-133.

Shu, H., Meng, X., Chen, X., Luan, H., & Cao, F. (2005). The subtypes of developmental dyslexia in Chinese:Evidence from three cases. Dyslexia, 11(4), 311-329.

Siok, W. T., Jin, Z., Fletcher, P., & Tan, L. H. (2003). Distinct brain regions associated with syllable and phoneme. Human Brain Mapping, 18(3), 201-207.

Snowling, M. J. (Ed). (2000). Dyslexia. Oxford:Blackwell.

Stanovich, K. E., Siegel, L. S., & Gottardo, A. (1997). Converging evidence for phonological and surface subtypes of reading disability. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89(1), 114-127.

Suárez-Coalla, P., Ramos, S., Álvarez-Cañizo, M., & Cuetos, F. (2014). Orthographic learning in dyslexic Spanish children. Annuals of Dyslexia, 64(2), 166-181.

Tallal, P. (1980). Language and reading:Some perceptual prerequisites. Annuals of Dyslexia, 30(1), 170-178.

Tallal, P. (2004). Improving language and literacy is a matter of time. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 5(9), 721-728

Tan, L. H., Liu, H.L., Perfetti, C. A., Spinks, J. A., Fox, P. T., & Gao, J.H. (2001). The neural system underlying Chinese logograph reading. NeuroImage, 13(5), 836-846.

Valdois, S., Grard, C., Vanault, P., & Dugas, M. (1995). Peripheral developmental dyslexia:a visual attentional account? Cognitive Neuropsychology, 12(1), 31-67.

Wagner, R. K., & Torgesen, J. K. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101(2), 192-212.

Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., Rashotte, C. A., Hecht, S. A., Barker, T. A., Burgess, S. R., et al. (1997). Changing relations between phonological processing abilities and word-level reading as children develop from beginning to skilled readers:a 5-year longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 33(3), 468-479.

Wang, L. C., & Yang, H. M. (2014). Classifying Chinese children with dyslexia by dual-route and triangle models of Chinese reading. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 35(11), 2702-2713.

Wolf, M. (1999). What time may tell:Towards a new conceptualization of developmental dyslexia. Annuals of Dyslexia, 49(1), 1-28.

Wolf, M., & Bowers, P. G. (1999). The double-deficit hypothesis for the developmental dyslexias. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91(3), 415-438.

Yin, W., & Weekes, B. (2003). Dyslexia in Chinese:Clues from cognitive neuropsychology. Annuals of Dyslexia, 53(1), 255-279.

Zhao, J., Qian, Y., Bi, H. Y., & Coltheart, M. (2014). The visual magnocellular-dorsal dysfunction in Chinese children with developmental dyslexia impedes Chinese character recognition. Scientific Reports, 4(1), 7068-7072.

Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages:a psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131(1), 3-29.

Ziegler, J. C., Castela, C., Pech-Georgelb, C., Georgeb, F., Alarioa, F.X., & Perryc, C. (2008). Developmental dyslexia and the dual route model of reading:Simulating individual differences and subtypes. Cognition, 107(1), 151-178.

曹思娜, 舒华. (2007). 发展性阅读障碍亚类型的研究. 中国特殊教育, 82(4), 28-34.

傅永和. (1991). 汉字的部件. 语文建设, (12), 3-6.

刘文理, 刘翔平. (2006). 阅读发展相关的认知技能:汉语和英语的比较. 心理科学进展, 14(5), 665-674.

刘文理, 刘翔平, 张婧乔. (2006). 汉语发展性阅读障碍亚类型的初步探讨. 心理学报, 38(5), 681-694.

栾辉, 舒华, 黎程正家, 林薇. (2002). 汉语发展性深层阅读障碍的个案研究. 心理学报, 34(4), 338-343.

舒华,毕雪梅,武宁宁. (2003). 声旁部分信息在儿童学习和记忆汉字中的作用. 心理学报, 35(1), 9-16.

肖茜,张逸玮,赵婧, 毕鸿燕. (2014). 汉语发展性阅读障碍儿童的视觉快速加工能力. 中国心理卫生杂志, 28(9), 679-684.

张承芬, 张景焕, 殷荣生, 周静, 常淑敏. (1996). 关于我国学生汉语阅读困难的研究. 心理科学, 19(4), 222-226.

赵婧, 毕鸿燕, 杨炀. (2012). 汉语发展性阅读障碍儿童的快速命名与正字法加工技能. 中国心理卫生杂志, 26(1), 36-40.
[1] 孟红霞, 白学军, 谭珂, 苏娟. 发展性阅读障碍的视觉拥挤效应[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2020, 36(6): 762-768.
[2] 刘涵隆, 赵婧. 汉语发展性阅读障碍儿童视觉注意广度的发展及其对阅读流畅性的作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2018, 34(5): 533-540.
[3] 李何慧, 陶伍海, 彭聃龄, 丁国盛. 发展性阅读障碍与脑异常的因果关系:研究范式及发现[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2017, 33(5): 631-640.
[4] 王晓辰, 李其维, 李清. 小学汉语阅读障碍儿童的PASS认知加工特点[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2011, 27(6): 569-576.
[5] 刘文理, 伊廷伟, 杨玉芳. 汉语语音型阅读障碍儿童的范畴知觉技能[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2010, 26(6): 569-576.
[6] 金颖, 刘翔平, 张微, 兰彦婷. 激活水平对两亚型ADHD儿童反应执行与抑制能力的影响[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2009, 25(4): 81-86.
[7] 刘卫卫, 刘翔平, 钟姝. 不同亚型ADHD儿童的视通道时距估计能力[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2008, 24(3): 30-35.
[8] 郑淑杰. 9岁行为退缩儿童亚类型[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2008, 24(3): 26-29,35.
[9] 张树东, 董奇. 一~四年级小学生发展性计算障碍的亚类型研究[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2007, 23(2): 76-81.
[10] 田晓梅, 王纯, 李童, 孟祥芝, 周晓林. 汉语双字词的心理词典存储—来自发展性阅读障碍个案的证据[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2006, 22(2): 46-51.
[11] 吴思娜, 舒华, 王彧. 4~6年级小学生发展性阅读障碍的异质性研究[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2004, 20(3): 46-50.
[12] 刘翔平, 候典牧, 杨双, 杜雯翀. 阅读障碍儿童汉字认知特点研究[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2004, 20(2): 7-11.
[13] 曾飚, 周晓林, 孟祥芝. 发展性阅读障碍的注意缺陷研究现状[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2003, 18(2): 91-95.
[14] 徐世勇, 彭聃龄, 薛贵, 谭力海. 汉语发展性阅读障碍儿童心理机制的初步研究[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2001, 16(4): 12-16,22.
Viewed
Full text


Abstract

Cited

  Shared   
  Discussed   
No Suggested Reading articles found!