心理发展与教育 ›› 2014, Vol. 30 ›› Issue (5): 533-539.

• 心理健康与教育 • 上一篇    下一篇

网络游戏成瘾者注意焦点转换功能的特异性损伤研究

张豹1, 黄赛2, 侯秋霞3   

  1. 1. 广州大学心理学系, 广州 510006;
    2. 华南师范大学心理学院, 广州 510631;
    3. 嘉应学院教育科学学院, 梅州 514015
  • 出版日期:2014-09-15 发布日期:2014-09-15
  • 通讯作者: 侯秋霞,E-mail:houqiuxia2005@126.com E-mail:houqiuxia2005@126.com
  • 基金资助:
    广东省哲学社科规划项目青年项目(GD11YXL02);广州市哲学社科规划项目(13Q19);广州市教育科学规划项目(11A167).

The Deficit Characteristics of Attentional Focus Switching Function in Online Game Addicts

ZHANG Bao1, HUANG Sai2, HOU Qiu-xia3   

  1. 1. Department of Psychology, Guangzhou University, Guangzhou 510006;
    2. School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510631;
    3. School of Education Science, Jiaying University, Meizhou 514015
  • Online:2014-09-15 Published:2014-09-15

摘要: 网络游戏成瘾对人类认知功能的损伤日益成为研究焦点之一。采用注意焦点转换任务,分别要求网络游戏成瘾被试与正常被试在工作记忆的语音环内、视空画板内及语音环——视空画板间进行注意焦点转换,然后比较两组被试在各转换任务中转换代价的差异,来探讨网络游戏成瘾者的注意焦点转换功能是否受损以及受损的特征。结果发现网络游戏成瘾组被试在视空画板内进行注意焦点转换时比对照组被试表现出更大的转换代价,而在其他两种转换任务中则没有发现组间差异。此结果表明网络游戏成瘾者的注意转换功能受到特异性损伤。

关键词: 网络游戏成瘾, 注意焦点转换, 特异性损伤

Abstract: The characteristic of cognitive deficits in the online game addicts is one of hot issues concerned by numerous researchers. Here, adopting the attentional focus task, we Required 14 game addicts and 14 normal participants to perform attentional focus switching task within phonological loop, within visuospatial sketchpad and between them, then compared the switching cost between two groups. The results showed that the switching cost was significant greater for game addicts than normal participants only in the task of switching within visuospatial sketchpad, however, no significant differences was found in the other switching task. These results indicated that there was a specific deficit on the attentional focus switching function for the online game addicts.

Key words: online game addict, attentional focus switching function, specific cognitive deficits

中图分类号: 

  • B844.3
Abi-Dargham, A., Mawlawi, O., Lombardo, I., Gil, R., Martinez, D., Huang, Y., et al. (2002). Prefrontal dopamine D1 receptors and working memory in schizophrenia. The Journal of Neuroscience, 22(9), 3708-3719.
Achtman, R. L., Green, C. S., & Bavelier, D. (2008). Video games as a tool to train visual skills. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, 26(4-5), 435-446.
Bailey, K., West, R., & Anderson, C. A. (2010). A negative association between video game experience and proactive cognitive control. Psychophysiology, 47(1), 34-42.
Bartholow, B. D., Bushman, B. J., & Sestir, M. A. (2006). Chronic violent video game exposure and desensitization to violence: Behavioral and event-related brain potential data. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42(4), 532-539.
Berke, J. D., & Hyman, S. E. (2000). Addiction, dopamine, and the molecular mechanisms of memory. Neuron, 25(3), 515-532.
Boot, W. R., Kramer, A. F., Simons, D. J., Fabiani, M., & Gratton, G. (2008). The effects of video game playing on attention, memory, and executive control. Acta Psychologica, 129(3), 387-398.
Colzato, L. S., Van Leeuwen, P. J., van den Wildenberg, W. P., & Hommel, B. (2010). DOOM'd to switch: superior cognitive flexibility in players of first person shooter games. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 1-5.
Costa, A., Peppe, A., Dell'Agnello, G., Carlesimo, G. A., Murri, L., Bonuccelli, U., & Caltagirone, C. (2003). Dopaminergic modulation of visual-spatial working memory in Parkinson's disease. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders 15(2), 55-66.
Decker, S. A., & Gay, J. N. (2011). Cognitive-bias toward gaming-related words and disinhibition in World of Warcraft gamers. Computers in Human Behavior, 27(2), 798-810.
Dong, G., Zhou, H., & Zhao, X. (2011). Male Internet addicts show impaired executive control ability: Evidence from a color-word Stroop task. Neuroscience Letters, 499(2), 114-118.
Dunlop, B. W., & Nemeroff, C. B. (2007). The role of dopamine in the pathophysiology of depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 64(3), 327-337.
Gentile, D. (2009). Pathological Video-Game Use Among Youth Ages 8 to 18: A National Study. Psychological Science, 20(5), 594-602.
Gerdeman, G. L., Partridge, J. G., Lupica, C. R., & Lovinger, D. M. (2003). It could be habit forming: drugs of abuse and striatal synaptic plasticity. Trends in Neurosciences, 26(4), 184-192.
Glickstein, S. B., Hof, P. R., & Schmauss, C. (2002). Mice lacking dopamine D2 and D3 receptors have spatial working memory deficits. The Journal of Neuroscience, 22(13), 5619-5629.
Green, C. S., Sugarman, M. A., Medford, K., Klobusicky, E., & Daphne, B. (2012). The effect of action video game experience on task-switching. Computers in Human Behavior, 28(3), 984-994.
Griffiths, M. D. (2000). Does Internet and computer "addiction" exist? Some case study evidence. CyberPsychology and Behavior, 3(2), 211-218.
Griffiths, M. D. (2008). Videogame addiction: Further thoughts and observations. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 6(2), 182-185.
Han, D. H., et al. (2011). Brain activity and desire for Internet video game play. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 52(1), 88-95.
Han, D. H., Lee, Y. S., Yang, K. C., Kim, E. Y., Lyoo, I. K., & Renshaw, P. F. (2007). Dopamine genes and reward dependence in adolescents with excessive internet video game play. Journal of Addiction Medicine, 1(3), 133-138.
Johansson, A., & Götestam, K. G. (2004). Problems with computer games without monetary reward: Similarity to pathological gambling. Psychological Reports, 95(2), 641-650.
Kapur, S., & John M. J. (1992). Role of the dopaminergic system in depression. Biological psychiatry, 32(1), 1-17.
Karle, J. W., Watter, S., & Shedden, J. M. (2010). Task switching in video game players: Benefits of selective attention but not resistance to proactive interference. Acta Psychologica, 134(1), 70-78.
Kirsh, S. J., Olczak, P. V., & Mounts, J. R. W. (2005). Violent video games induce an affect processing bias. Media Psychology, 7(3), 239-250.
Koepp M. J., et al. Evidence for striatal dopamine release during a video game. Nature, 1998, 393(6682): 266-268.
Kübler, A., Murphy, K., & Garavan, H. (2005). Cocaine dependence and attention switching within and between verbal and visuospatial working memory. European Journal of Neuroscience, 21(7), 1984-1992.
Kübler, A., Murphy, K., Kaufman, J., Stein, E. A., & Garavan, H. (2003). Co-ordination within and between verbal and visuospatial working memory: network modulation and anterior frontal recruitment. NeuroImage, 20(2), 1298-1308.
Kuss, D., & Griffiths, M. (2012). Online gaming addiction in children and adolescents: A review of empirical research. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 1(1), 3-22.
Mathews, V. P., Kronenberger, W. G., Wang, Y., Lurito, J. T., Lowe, M. J., & Dunn, D. W. (2005). Media violence exposure and frontal lobe activation measured by functional magnetic resonance imaging in aggressive and nonaggressive adolescents. Journal of Computer Assisted Tomography, 29(3), 287-292.
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex "frontal lobe" tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49-100.
Monsell, S. (2003). Task switching. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(3), 134-140.
Murphy, B. L., Arnsten, A. F., Goldman-Rakic, P., & Roth, R. H. (1996). Increased dopamine turnover in the prefrontal cortex impairs spatial working memory performance in rats and monkeys. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 93(3), 1325-1329.
Murphy, B. L., Arnsten, A. F., Jentsch, J. D., & Roth, R. H. (1996). Dopamine and spatial working memory in rats and monkeys: pharmacological reversal of stress-induced impairment. The Journal of neuroscience, 16(23), 7768-7775.
Oberauer, K. (2002). Access to information in working memory: Exploring the focus of attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28(3), 411-421.
Olson, C. K. (2010). Children's motivations for video game play in the context of normal development. Review of General Psychology, 14(2), 180.
Owen, A. M., Iddon, J. L., Hodges, J. R., Summers, B. A., & Robbins, T. W. (1997). Spatial and non-spatial working memory at different stages of Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychologia, 35(4), 519-532.
Park, S., & Holzman, P. S. (1992). Schizophrenics show spatial working memory deficits. Archives of General Psychiatry, 49(12), 975-982.
Prot, S., McDonald, K. A., Anderson, C. A., & Gentile, D. A. (2012). Video Games: Good, Bad, or Other? Pediatric Clinics of North America, 59(3), 647-658.
Sawaguchi, T., & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1991). D1 dopamine receptors in prefrontal cortex: involvement in working memory. Science, 251(4996), 947-950.
Swing, E. L., Gentile, D. A., Anderson, C. A., & Walsh, D. A. (2010). Television and video game exposure and the development of attention problems. Pediatrics, 126(2), 214-221.
Unsworth, N., & Engle, R. W. (2008). Speed and accuracy of accessing information in working memory: an individual differences investigation of focus switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34(3), 616-630.
Verhaeghen, P., & Hoyer, W. J. (2007). Aging, focus switching, and task switching in a continuous calculation task: evidence toward a new working memory control process. Neuropsychology, Development, and Cognition. Section B, Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition, 14(1), 22-39.
Vijayraghavan S., et al. Inverted-U dopamine D1 receptor actions on prefrontal neurons engaged in working memory. Nature Neuroscience, 2007, 10(3), 376-384.
Wan, C. S., & Chiou, W. B. (2006). Psychological Motives and Online Games Addiction: A Test of Flow Theory and Humanistic Needs Theory for Taiwanese Adolescents. Cyberpsychology & Behavior, 9(3), 317-324.
Weinstein, A. M. (2010). Computer and video game addiction-a comparison between game users and non-game users. The American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 36(5), 268-276.
Williams, G., & Goldmanrakic, P. (1995). Modulation of Memory Fields by Dopamine D1 Receptors in Prefrontal Cortex. Nature, 376(6541), 572-575.
Zahrt, J., Taylor, J. R., Mathew, R. G., & Arnsten, A. F. (1997). Supranormal stimulation of D1 dopamine receptors in the rodent prefrontal cortex impairs spatial working memory performance. The Journal of neuroscience, 17(21), 8528-8535.
崔丽娟, 胡海龙, 吴明证, 解春玲. (2006). 网络游戏成瘾者的内隐攻击性研究. 心理科学, 29(3), 570-573.
戴砷懿, 马庆国, 王小毅. (2012). 网络游戏成瘾者对成瘾相关线索的注意偏向: 一项 ERP 研究. 心理科学, 34(6), 1302-1307.
贺金波, 郭永玉, 柯善玉, 赵仑. (2008). 网络游戏成瘾者认知功能损害的 ERP 研究. 心理科学, 31(2), 380-384.
黄赛. (2010). 抑郁症状个体转换功能受损的行为学及ERP研究. 硕士学位论文, 第三军医大学, 重庆.
黄赛, 冯正直. (2010). 抑郁情绪个体执行功能的转换成分缺失研究. 中华行为医学与脑科学杂志, 19(4), 372-373.
魏华, 张丛丽, 周宗奎, 金琼, 田媛. (2010). 媒体暴力对大学生攻击性的长时效应和短时效应. 心理发展与教育, 5, 489-494.
魏华, 周宗奎, 田媛, 鲍娜. (2012). 网络游戏成瘾: 沉浸的影响及其作用机制. 心理发展与教育, 28(006), 651-657.
肖琳, 谭北平, 李勇辉, 隋南. (2005). 成瘾现象中的奖赏效应和神经系统适应性. 中国药物依赖性杂志, 13(4), 241-244.
张豹, 黄赛, 冯正直. (2013). 游戏成瘾者的转换功能损伤研究. 重庆医学, 42(29), 3527-3531.
[1] 林悦, 刘勤学, 余思, 周宗奎. 父母忽视与青少年网络游戏成瘾的关系:希望的中介作用和性别的调节作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2021, 37(1): 109-119.
[2] 宫剑, 梁晓燕, 刘晓飞. 神经质人格与MMORPG游戏成瘾的关系:链式中介效应分析[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2020, 36(1): 113-120.
[3] 田云龙, 喻承甫, 林霜, 叶诗敏, 张晓琳, 刘毅, 路红, 张卫. 父母体罚、学校参与与青少年网络游戏成瘾:亲子关系的调节作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2018, 34(4): 461-471.
[4] 苏斌原, 张卫, 苏勤, 喻承甫. 父母网络监管对青少年网络游戏成瘾为何事与愿违?——一个有调节的中介效应模型[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2016, 32(5): 604-613.
[5] 鲍学峰, 张卫, 喻承甫, 朱键军, 鲍振宙, 江艳平, 马娜, 吴涛, 陈遵炳. 初中生感知校园氛围与网络游戏成瘾的关系:学业自我效能感的中介效应与父母学业卷入的调节效应[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2016, 32(3): 358-368.
[6] 张国华, 雷雳. 基于技术接受模型的青少年网络游戏成瘾机制研究[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2015, 31(4): 437-444.
[7] 魏华, 周宗奎, 田媛, 鲍娜. 网络游戏成瘾:沉浸的影响及其作用机制[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2012, 28(6): 651-657.
Viewed
Full text


Abstract

Cited

  Shared   
  Discussed   
No Suggested Reading articles found!