心理发展与教育 ›› 2024, Vol. 40 ›› Issue (6): 782-791.doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2024.06.03
• 认知与社会性发展 • 上一篇
崔佳歆1,2, 冯佳佳1, 左钰涵1, 崔占玲1, 周新林2
CUI Jiaxin1,2, FENG Jiajia1, ZUO Yuhan1, CUI Zhanling1, ZHOU Xinlin2
摘要: 最新提出的形状知觉假说认为形状知觉是语言和数学共有的认知基础, 已有研究考察了形状知觉在不同类型数学能力中的作用, 但其在不同类型和层次的语言能力中的作用尚不清楚。本研究选取小学5年级学生, 考察形状知觉在不同类型不同层次语言能力中的独立性贡献。结果发现, 控制了年龄和性别两个人口学变量以及智力和反应速度两个基础认知能力后, 形状知觉对词汇水平的语音加工、正字法意识和语义理解以及句子水平上的阅读理解有显著的独立性贡献, 而对篇章水平上的阅读理解无显著贡献。结果说明形状知觉在个体可以快速加工的语言流畅性能力中发挥显著作用, 在非流畅性的复杂语言能力中无显著作用。
中图分类号:
Ahmed, M., Ward, R., & Kharma, N. (2004). Solving mathematical problems using knowledge-based systems. Mathematics and Computers in Simulation, 67(1-2), 149-161. Anderson, J. M. (2006). Structural analogy and universal grammar. Lingua, 116(5), 601-633. Anobile, G., Stievano, P., & Burr, D. C. (2013). Visual sustained attention and numerosity sensitivity correlate with math achievement in children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116(2), 380-391. Atagi, N., DeWolf, M., Stigler, J. W., & Johnson, S. P. (2016). The role of visual representations in college students' understanding of mathematical notation. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Applied, 22(3), 295-304. Balakrishnan, G., Uppinakudru, G., Girwar Singh, G., Bangera, S., Dutt Raghavendra, A., & Thangavel, D. (2014). A comparative study on visual choice reaction time for different colors in females. Neurology Research International, 2014, 1-5. https://doi.org/10.1155/2014/301473 Bauer, D. J., Goldfifield, B. A., & Reznik, J. S. (2002). Alternative approaches to analyzing individual differences in the rate of early vocabulary acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 23, 313-335. Boets, B., Vandermosten, M., Cornelissen, P., Wouters, J., & Ghesquiere, P. (2011). Coherent motion sensitivity and reading development in the transition from prereading to reading stage. Child Development, 82(3), 854-869. Cavina-Pratesi, C., Large, M.-E., & Milner, A. D. (2015). Reprint of: Visual processing of words in a patient with visual form agnosia: A behavioural and fMRI study. Cortex, 72, 97-114. Cheng, D., Xiao, Q., Chen, Q., Cui, J., & Zhou, X. (2018). Dyslexia and are characterized by common visual perception deficits. Developmental Neuropsychology, 43(6), 497-507. Cheng, D., Xiao, Q., Cui, J., Chen, C., Zeng, J., Chen, Q., & Zhou, X. (2020). Short-term numerosity training promotes symbolic arithmetic in children with developmental dyscalculia: The mediating role of visual form perception. Developmental Science, 23(4), e12910. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12910 Child, A. E., Cirino, P. T., Fletcher, J. M., Willcutt, E. G., & Fuchs, L. S. (2019). A cognitive dimensional approach to understanding shared and unique contributions to reading, Math, and Attention Skills. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 52(1), 15-30. Conlon, E. G., Sanders, M. A., & Wright, C. M. (2009). Relationships between global motion and global form processing, practice, cognitive and visual processing in adults with dyslexia or visual discomfort. Neuropsychologia, 47(3), 907-915. Cui, J., Xiao, R., Ma, M., Yuan, L., Kodash, R. C., & Zhou, X. (2020). Children skilled in mental abacus show enhanced non-symbolic number sense. Current Psychology, 41(4), 2053-2066. Cui, J., Zhang, Y., Cheng, D., Li, D., & Zhou, X. (2017). Visual form perception can be a cognitive correlate of lower level math categories for teenagers. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1336. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01336 Cui, J., Zhang, Y., Wan, S., Chen, C., Zeng, J., & Zhou, X. (2019). Visual form perception is fundamental for both reading comprehension and arithmetic computation. Cognition, 189, 141-154. Druks, J., & Froud, K. (2002). The syntax of single words: Evidence from a patient with a selective function word reading deficit. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 19(3), 207-244. Erath, K., Prediger, S., Quasthoff, U., & Heller, V. (2018). Discourse competence as important part of academic language proficiency in mathematics classrooms: The case of explaining to learn and learning to explain. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 99(2), 161-179. Fletcher, J. M. (2006). Measuring reading comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10(3), 323-330. Ford, B. (2004). Parsing expression grammars. ACM SIGPLAN Notices, 39(1), 111-122. Friedman, R. B., & Lott, S. N. (2000). Rapid word identification in pure alexia is lexical but not semantic. Brain and Language, 72(3), 219-237. Fujii, K., & Suda, T. (2009). Semantics-based context-aware dynamic service composition. ACM Transactions on Autonomous and Adaptive Systems, 4(2), 1-31. Geary, D. C., Hoard, M. K., Nugent, L., Unal, Z. E., & Scofield, J. E. (2020). Comorbid learning difficulties in reading and mathematics: The role of intelligence and in-Class attentive behavior. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 572099. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.572099 James, T. W., Culham, J., Humphrey, G. K., AD Milner, & Goodale, M. A. (2003). Ventral occipital lesions impair object recognition but not object-directed grasping: An fMRI study. Brain, 126(Pt 11), 2463-2475. Kaushanskaya, M., Marian, V., & Yoo, J. (2011). Gender differences in adult word learning. Acta Psychologica, 137(1), 24-35. Kaushanskaya, M., Gross, M., & Buac, M. (2013). Gender differences in child word learning. Learning and Individual Differences, 27, 82-89. Kim, J. G., Biederman, I., Lescroart, M. D., & Hayworth, K. J. (2009). Adaptation to objects in the lateral occipital complex (LOC): Shape or semantics? Vision Research, 49(18), 2297-2305. Kintsch, W., & van Dijk, T. (1978). Toward a model of discourse comprehension and production. Psychological Review, 85, 363-394. Kourtzi, Z., & Kanwisher, N. (2001). Representation of perceived object shape by the human lateral occipital complex. Science, 293(5534), 1506-1509. Liang, Z. H., Dong, P. Q., Zhou, Y. L., Feng, S. S., & Zhang, Q. (2022). Whether verbal and visuospatial working memory play different roles in pupil's mathematical abilities. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 92(2), 409-424. Liu, D., Chen, X., & Wang, Y. (2016). The impact of visual-spatial attention on reading and spelling in Chinese children. Reading and Writing, 29(7), 1435-1447. Liu, H., Jiang, Y., Zhang, B., Ma, L., He, S., & Weng, X. (2013). The orthographic sensitivity to written Chinese in the occipital-temporal cortex. Experimental Brain Research, 227(3), 387-396. Liu, N., Zhao, J., Huang, C., Xing, X., Lu, S., & Wang, Z. (2021). Predicting early reading fluency based on preschool measures of low-level visual temporal processing: A possible mediation by high-level visual temporal processing skills. Infant and Child Development, 30(2), e2211. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.2211 Lu, Y., Ma, M., Chen, G., & Zhou, X. (2021). Can abacus course eradicate developmental dyscalculia. Psychology in the Schools, 58(2), 235-251. Lynn, R. (2010). High IQ is sufficient to explain the high achievements in math and science of the East Asian peoples. Learning and Individual Differences, 20(6), 567-568. Malach, R., Reppas, J. B., Benson, R. R., Kwong, K. K., Jiang, H., Kennedy, W. A., … Tootell, R. B. (1995). Object-related activity revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging in human occipital cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 92(18), 8135-8139. Marantz, A. (2013). No escape from morphemes in morphological processing. Language and Cognitive Processes, 28(7), 905-916. Massa, L. J., & Mayer, R. E.. (2006). Testing the ati hypothesis: should multimedia instruction accommodate verbalizer-visualizer cognitive style? Learning & Individual Differences, 16(4), 321-335. Mayes, S. D., & Calhoun, S. L.. (2006). Frequency of reading, math, and writing disabilities in children with clinical disorders. Learning & Individual Differences, 16(2), 145-157. Milner, A. D., Perrett, D. I., Johnston, R. S., Benson, P. J., Jordan, T. R., Heeley, D. W., … Terazzi, E. (1991). Perception and action in 'visual form agnosia'. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 114 (Pt 1B), 405-428. Nakamura, K., Honda, M., Okada, T., Hanakawa, T., Toma, K., Fukuyama, H., … Shibasaki, H. (2000). Participation of the left posterior inferior temporal cortex in writing and mental recall of kanji orthography: A functional MRI study. Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 123 (Pt 5), 954-967. Neudorf, J., Ekstrand, C., Kress, S., & Borowsky, R. (2019). FMRI of shared-stream priming of lexical identification by object semantics along the ventral visual processing stream. Neuropsychologia, 133, 107185. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2019.107185 Noppeney, U., Price, C. J., Duncan, J. S., & Koepp, M. J. (2005). Reading skills after left anterior temporal lobe resection: An fMRI study. Brain, 128, 1377-1385. Osborne, T., & Gross, T. (2012). Constructions are catenae: Construction grammar meets dependency grammar. Cognitive Linguistics, 23(1), 165-216. Passolunghi, M. C., & Lanfranchi, S. (2012). Domain-specific and domain-general precursors of mathematical achievement: A longitudinal study from kindergarten to first grade. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 82(1), 42-63. Perfetti, C. A., & Hogaboam, T. (1975). The relationship between single word decoding and reading comprehension skill. Journal of Educational Psychology, 67,461-469. Pitcher, D., & Ungerleider, L. G. (2021). Evidence for a third visual pathway specialized for social perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 25(2), 100-110. Price, C. J., & Devlin, J. T. (2003). The myth of the visual word form area. Neuroimage, 19(3), 473-481. Qian, Y., Bi, Y., Wang, X., Zhang, Y.-W., & Bi, H.-Y. (2016). Visual dorsal stream is associated with Chinese reading skills: A resting-state fMRI study. Brain and Language, 160, 42-49. Schneider, W. J., & McGrew, K. S. (2012). The Cattell-Horn-Carroll model of intelligence. In D. P. Flanagan & P. L. Harrison (Eds.), Contemporary intellectual assessment: Theories, tests, and issues (3rd ed., pp. 99-144). New York: Guilford Press. Shtyrov, Y. (2011). Fast mapping of novel word forms traced neurophysiologically. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 340. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00340 Sigmundsson, H., Anholt, S. K., & Talcott, J. B. (2010). Are poor mathematics skills associated with visual deficits in temporal processing? Neuroscience Letters, 469(2), 248-250. Soltesz, F., & Szucs, D. (2014). Neural adaptation to non-symbolic number and visual shape: An electrophysiological study. Biological Psychology, 103, 203-211. Song, H., & An, J. (2017). The relationship between executive functions and IQ in Korean children and the comparison with Chinese children. Applied Neuropsychology-Child, 6(2), 158-165. Szwed, M., Qiao, E., Jobert, A., Dehaene, S., & Cohen, L. (2014). Effects of literacy in early visual and occipitotemporal areas of Chinese and French readers. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 26(3), 459-475. Tibber, M. S., Manasseh, G. S. L., Clarke, R. C., Gagin, G., Swanbeck, S. N., Butterworth, B., … Dakin, S. C. (2013). Sensitivity to numerosity is not a unique visuospatial psychophysical predictor of mathematical ability. Vision Research, 89, 1-9. Tong, X., Leung, W. W. S., & Tong, X. (2019). Visual statistical learning and orthographic awareness in Chinese children with and without developmental dyslexia. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 92, 103443. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2019.103443 Wang, L., Sun, Y., & Zhou, X. (2016). Relation between approximate number system acuity and mathematical achievement: The influence of fluency. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1966. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01966 Wang, L.-C., & Yang, H.-M. (2019). The roles of various forms of attention in temporal processing deficits in Chinese children with and without dyslexia. Learning Disability Quarterly, 43(4), 241-253. Yang, X., & Meng, X. (2020). Visual processing matters in Chinese reading acquisition and early mathematics. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 462. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00462 Yang, J., & Tan, L. H. (2020). Whole-Brain functional networks for phonological and orthographic processing in Chinese good and poor readers. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 2945. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02945 Zhang, Y., Chen, C., Liu, H., Cui, J., & Zhou, X. (2016). Both non-symbolic and symbolic quantity processing are important for arithmetical computation but not for mathematical reasoning. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 28(7), 807-824. Zhang, B., He, S., & Weng, X. (2018). Localization and functional characterization of an occipital visual word form sensitive area. Scientific Reports, 8, 6723. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-25029-z Zhang, Y., Liu, T., Chen, C., & Zhou, X. (2019). Visual form perception supports approximate number system acuity and arithmetic fluency. Learning and Individual Differences, 71, 1-12. Zhou, X., & Cheng, D. (2015). When and why numerosity processing is associated with developmental dyscalculia. In S. Chinn (Ed.). The Routledge international handbook of dyscalculia and mathematical learning difficulties (pp. 78-89). New York: Routledge. Zhou, X., Hu, Y., Yuan, L., Gu, T., & Li, D. (2020). Visual form perception predicts 3-year longitudinal development of mathematical achievement. Cognitive Processing, 21(4), 521-532. Zhou, X., Wei, W., Zhang, Y., Cui, J., & Chen, C. (2015). Visual perception can account for the close relation between numerosity processing and computational fluency. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1364. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01364 陈宝国,王立新,彭聃龄.(2003).汉字识别中形音义激活时间进程的研究(Ⅱ). 心理学报, (05), 576-581. 彭聃龄,徐世勇,丁国盛,李恩中,刘颖. (2003). 汉语单字词音、义加工的脑激活模式. 中国神经科学杂志, (05), 287-291. 徐世勇, 彭聃龄, 薛贵, 谭力海. (2001). 汉语发展性阅读障碍儿童心理机制的初步研究. 心理发展与教育, 17(4), 12-16. 赵婧. (2019). 发展性阅读障碍的视觉注意广度技能. 心理科学进展, 27(01), 20-26. |
[1] | 刘仕凡, 张蓉, 王美芳, 方永超, 赵建设. 长期经历父母体罚儿童的自发推理特点[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(5): 616-624. |
[2] | 刘倩文, 高晗晶, 王振宏. 亲子关系与CLOCK基因rs1801260多态性对学前儿童亲社会行为的交互影响:性别的调节作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(5): 625-635. |
[3] | 谢明珺, 孙佳宁, 肖家乐, 弓鑫钰, 林丹华. 从累加到类型视角:儿童期逆境对个体神经生理发展的影响[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(5): 737-749. |
[4] | 张环, 谢忠珖, 王亚丽, 秦锡权, 刘拓. 学龄中期儿童团体创造力的发展特点及其与个体创造力的关系[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(4): 468-478. |
[5] | 牛骅, 王美芳. 父母体罚与儿童执行功能的关系:一项3年的追踪研究[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(4): 479-487. |
[6] | 黄鹤, 王小英. 正念教养与学前儿童问题行为的关系:亲子关系与心理弹性的链式中介[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(4): 533-541. |
[7] | 辛聪, 张曼曼. 自闭症儿童前瞻记忆的影响因素及改善策略[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(4): 589-600. |
[8] | 邓之君, 朱小爽, 张和颐. 远虑解近忧——儿童青少年的未来时间洞察及其影响[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(4): 601-608. |
[9] | 王娟, 刘天碧, 范小月. 象似性手势和指示性手势对自闭症儿童动作相关词汇学习的影响[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(3): 367-374. |
[10] | 储月, 刘希平, 徐慧, 唐卫海. 儿童社会分享型提取诱发遗忘的发展特点[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(2): 153-159. |
[11] | 刘艳春, 邓玉婷, 张曦. 智力障碍儿童对不同对象的分享行为:心理理论的作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(2): 160-168. |
[12] | 梁丹丹, 闫晓民, 葛志林. 4~8岁汉语高功能自闭症儿童基于语言线索的情绪识别能力发展研究[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(2): 169-175. |
[13] | 史梦梦, 任桂琴, 孙军红, 张鑫星. 词汇类型和阅读水平对小学一年级儿童阅读理解监控的影响[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(2): 207-214. |
[14] | 冷欣怡, 苏萌萌, 李文玲, 杨秀杰, 邢爱玲, 张湘琳, 舒华. 家庭环境与农村儿童早期语言发展的关系[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(1): 8-18. |
[15] | 赵京伟, 陈晓旭, 任立文, 耿喆, 徐夫真. 父母心理控制与小学儿童焦虑:一个有调节的中介模型[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(1): 93-102. |
|