Psychological Development and Education ›› 2017, Vol. 33 ›› Issue (6): 666-674.doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2017.06.04
Previous Articles Next Articles
DING Fengqin1, WANG Ximei1, LIU Zhao1,2
Bargh, J. A., & Shalev, I. (2012). The substitutability of physical and social warmth in daily life. Emoti on, 12(1), 154-162.Cramwinckel, F. M., Cremer, D., & Dijke, M. H. V. (2013). Dirty hands make dirty leaders? how the touching of dirty objects leads to higher rewards for unethical followers. Journal of Business Ethics, 115(1), 93-100.Dijksterhuis, A., & Bargh, J. A. (2001). The perception-behavior expressway:Automatic effects of social perception on social behavior. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 33(1), 1-40.Helzer, E. G., & Pizarro, D. A. (2011). Dirty liberals! reminders of physical cleanliness influence moral and political attitudes. Psychological Science, 22(4), 517-522.Holland, R. W., Hendriks, M., & Aarts, H. (2005). Smells Like Clean Spirit Nonconscious Effects of Scent on Cognition and Behavior. Psychological Science, 16(9), 689-693.Hong, J., & Sun, Y. (2012). Warm it up with love:The effect of physical coldness on liking of romance movies. Journal of Consumer Research, 39(2), 293-306.Ijzerman, H., Gallucci, M., Pouw, W. T. J. L., Weiβgerber, S. C., Doesum, N. J. V., & Williams, K. D. (2012). Cold-blooded loneliness:social exclusion leads to lower skin temperatures. Acta Psychologica, 140(3), 283-288.IJzerman, H., & Semin, G. R. (2009). The thermometer of social relations:Mapping social proximity on temperature. Psychological Science, 20(10), 1214-1220.Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the flesh:The embodied mind and its challenge to western thought. New York:Basic Books.Landau, M. J., Meier, B. P., & Keefer, L. A. (2010). A metaphor-enriched social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 136(6), 1045-1067.Lee, S. W. S., & Schwarz, N. (2010). Dirty hands and dirty mouths:Embodiment of the moral-purity metaphor is specific to the motor modality involved in moral transgression. Psychological Science, 21(10), 1423-1425.Liljenquist, K., Zhong, C. B., & Galinsky, A. D. (2010). The Smell of Virtue Clean Scents Promote Reciprocity and Charity. Psychological Science, 21(3), 381-383.Lu, A., Zhang, H., He, G., Zheng, D., & Hodges, B. H. (2014). Looking up to others:social status, chinese honorifics, and spatial attention. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 68(2), 77-83.Ma, Y., & Han, S. (2010). Why respond faster to the self than others? An implicit positive association theory of self advantage during implicit face recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Human Perception and Performance, 36(3), 619-633.Pecher, D., Boot, I., & Van Dantzig, S. (2011). Abstract concepts:Sensory-motor grounding, metaphors, and beyond. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 54, 217-248.Schnall, S., Haidt, J., Clore, G. L., & Jordan, A. H. (2008). Disgust as embodied moral judgment. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34(8), 1096-1109.Wang, Z., & Lu, Z. Y. (2011). A study on the metaphor of social exclusion from embodied cognition. Scientific Research and Essays, 6(10), 2225-2227.Wang, H. L., Lu, Y. Q., & Lu, Z. Y. (2016). Moral-up first, immoral-down last:the time course of moral metaphors on a vertical dimension. Neuroreport, 27 (4), 247-256.Wheeler, S. C., Morrison, K. R., Demarree, K. G., & Petty, R. E. (2008). Does self-consciousness increase or decrease priming effects? it depends. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(3), 882-889.Williams, L. E., & Bargh, J. A. (2008). Keeping One's distance the influence of spatial distance cues on affect and evaluation. Psychological Science, 19(3), 302-308.Williams, L. E., Huang, J. Y., & Bargh, J. A. (2009). The scaffolded mind:Higher mental processes are grounded in early experience of the physical world. European Journal of Social Psychology, 39(7), 1257-1267.Wu, X., Jia, H., Wang, E., Du, C., Wu, X., & Dang, C. (2016). Vertical position of chinese power words influences power judgments:evidence from spatial compatibility task and event-related potentials. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 102, 55-61.Zhong, C. B., & Liljenquist, K. (2006). Washing away your sins:threatened morality and physical cleansing. Science, 313(5792), 1451-1452.Zhong, C. B., Strejcek, B., & Sivanathan, N. (2010). A clean self can render harsh moral judgment. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(5), 859-862.陈玉明, 郭田友, 何立国, 燕良轼. (2014). 具身认知研究述评. 心理学探新, 34(6), 483-487.贾宁, 蒋高芳. (2016). 道德概念垂直空间隐喻的心理现实性及双向映射.心理发展与教育, 32(2), 158-165.黎晓丹, 杜建政, 叶浩生. (2016). 中国礼文化的具身隐喻效应:蜷缩的身体使人更卑微. 心理学报, 48(6), 746-756.玛丽·道格拉斯著, 黄剑波, 柳博赟, 卢忱. (2008). 洁净与危险. 北京:民族出版社, 142-157.唐佩佩, 叶浩生, 杜建政. (2015). 权力概念与空间大小:具身隐喻的视角.心理学报, 47(4), 514-521.王锃, 鲁忠义. (2013). 道德概念的垂直空间隐喻及其对认知的影响. 心理学报, 45(5), 538-545.吴念阳, 郝静. (2006). 以道德为本体的概念隐喻. 上海师范大学学报(基础教育版), 35(9), 51-55.叶浩生. (2010). 具身认知:认知心理学的新取向. 心理科学进展, 18(5), 705-710.殷融, 苏得权, 叶浩生. (2013). 具身认知视角下的概念隐喻理论. 心理科学进展, 21(2), 220-234.殷融, 叶浩生. (2014). 道德概念的黑白隐喻表征及其对道德认知的影响. 心理学报, 46(9), 1331-1346. |
[1] | DING Fengqin, SUN Yishu, Zhao Huying. The Bias of the Mapping in Clean and Dirty Metaphor of Moral Concepts [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2022, 38(3): 315-322. |
|