心理发展与教育 ›› 2022, Vol. 38 ›› Issue (1): 26-34.doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2022.01.04
吴瑕1,2,3, 钟希苹2, 姜云鹏1,2,3
WU Xia1,2,3, ZHONG Xiping2, JIANG Yunpeng1,2,3
摘要: 在视觉搜索过程中,老年人是否可以在不同情境下调节自上而下和自下而上的加工水平尚不明确。为了探讨老化对认知灵活性的影响,研究采用空间线索范式,通过比较有效线索和无效线索来考察对线索的注意加工水平(注意捕获、注意抑制),通过比较目标相关和目标无关线索的注意加工水平来考察自上而下和自下而上的作用,通过改变分心物一致性来调控不同的目标凸显情境。结果发现,年轻组在目标非凸显时的空间线索效应高于目标凸显时,表明年轻人会依据目标凸显性调整自上而下的加工水平;但老年组却没有产生这种差异,其更依赖自上而下的加工,注意系统会对目标相关的线索进行注意捕获,对目标无关的线索进行注意抑制。结果表明,随着年龄的增长,认知控制的灵活性下降,表现出对自上而下加工较多的依赖;此外,老年人依然保留着注意捕获和注意抑制的能力,这对认知老化中抑制能力受损的观点提出了挑战。
中图分类号:
Amenedo, E., Lorenzo-lópez, L., & Pazo-álvarez, P. (2012). Response processing during visual search in normal aging:The need for more time to prevent cross talk between spatial attention and manual response selection. Biological Psychology, 91(2), 201-211. Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Snyder, A. Z., Vincent, J. L., Lustig, C., Head, D.,Raichle, M. E., & Buckner, R. L. (2007). Disruption of large-scale brain systems in advanced aging. Neuron, 56, 924-935. Ansorge, U.,Priess, H., & Kerzel, D. (2013). Effects of relevant and irrelevant color singletons on inhibition of return and attentional capture. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 75(8), 1687-1702. Ashinoff, B. K., Geng, J. J., & Mevorach, C. (2019). Delayed Reactive Distractor Suppression in Aging Populations. Psychology and Aging, 34(3), 481-430. Bacon, W. F., & Egeth, H. E. (1994). Overriding stimulus-driven attentional capture. Perception & Psychophysics, 55(5), 485-496. Bélanger, S., Belleville, S., & Gauthier, S. (2010). Inhibition impairments in Alzheimer's disease, mild cognitive impairment and healthy aging:Effect of congruency proportion in a Stroop task. Neuropsychologia, 48(2), 581-590. Bennett, I. J.,Golob, E. J., & Starr, A. (2004). Age-related differences in auditory event-related potentials during a cued attention task. Clinical Neurophysiology, 115(13), 2602-2615. Braver, T. S. (2012). The variable nature of cognitive control:A dual mechanisms framework.Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(2), 106-113. Bruyer, R., & Brysbaert, M. (2011). Combining speed and accuracy in cognitive psychology:Is the inverse efficiency score (IES) a better dependent variable than the mean reaction time (RT) and the percentage of errors (PE)? Psychologica Belgica, 51(1), 5-13. Campbell, K. L., Grady, C. L., Ng, C., & Hasher, L. (2012). Age differences in the frontoparietal cognitive control network:Implications for distractibility.Neuropsychologia, 50(9), 2212-2223. Cassavaugh, N. D., Kramer, A. F., & Irwin, D. E. (2003). Influence of task-irrelevant onset distractors on the visual search performance of young and old adults. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 10(1), 44-60. Chou, Y., Chen, N., & Madden, D. J. (2013). Functional brain connectivity and cognition:Effects of adult age and task demands.Neurobiology of Aging, 34, 1925-1934. Cid-Fernández, S., LindÃ-n, M., & DÃ-az, F. (2014). Effects of aging and involuntary capture of attention on event-related potentials associated with the processing of and the response to a target stimulus. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8,745.https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00745 Davis, S. W., Dennis, N. A.,Daselaar, S. M., Fleck, M. S., & Cabeza, R. (2008). Que PASA? The posterior-anterior shift in aging. Cerebral Cortex, 18(5), 1201-1209. Deslauriers, J., Ansado, J., Marrelec, G., Provost, J. S., & Joanette, Y. (2017). Increase of posterior connectivity in aging within the Ventral Attention Network:A functional connectivity analysis using independent component analysis. Brain Research, 1657, 288-296. Eimer, M., & Kiss, M. (2010). Top-down search strategies determine attentional capture in visual search:Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 72(4), 951-962. Eimer, M., Kiss, M., & Cheung, T. (2010). Priming of pop-out modulates attentional target selection in visual search:Behavioural and electrophysiological evidence. Vision Research, 50(14), 1353-1361. Folk, C. L., & Hoyer, W. J. (1992). Aging and shifts of visual spatial attention. Psychology and Aging, 7(3), 453-465. Folk, C. L., & Remington, R. (1998). Selectivity in Distraction by Irrelevant Featural Singletons:Evidence for Two Forms of Attentional Capture.Journal of Experimental Psychology:Human Perception and Performance, 24(3), 847-858. Folk, C. L., & Remington, R. W. (2015). Unexpected abrupt onsets can override a top-down set for color.Journal of Experimental Psychology:Human Perception and Performance, 41(4), 1153-1165. Gazzaley, A., Clapp, W., Kelley, J.,Mcevoy, K., Knight, R. T., & Esposito, M. D. (2008). Age-related top-down suppression deficit in the early stages of cortical visual memory processing. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 105(35), 13122-13126. Hickey, C.,Lollo, V. Di, & Mcdonald, J. J. (2008). Electrophysiological indices of target and distractor processing in visual search. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(4), 760-775. Jernigan, T. L., Archibald, S. L.,Fennema-Notestine, C., Gamst, A. C., Stout, J. C., Bonner, J., & Hesselink, J. R. (2001). Effects of age on tissues and regions of the cerebrum and cerebellum. Neurobiology of Aging, 22(4), 581-594. Kawahara, J. (2010). Identifying a "default" visual search mode with operant conditioning. Acta Psychologica, 135(1), 38-49. Kiss, M.,Grubert, A., Petersen, A., & Eimer, M. (2012). Attentional capture by salient distractors during visual search is determined by temporal task demands. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24(3), 749-759. Kleerekooper, I., van Rooij, S. J. H., van Den Wildenberg, W. P. M., de Leeuw, M., Kahn, R. S., & Vink, M. (2016). The effect of aging on fronto-striatal reactive and proactive inhibitory control. NeuroImage, 132(2), 51-58. Kropotov, J., Ponomarev, V., Tereshchenko, E. P., Müller, A., & Jäncke, L. (2016). Effect of aging on ERP components of cognitive control. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 8, 69.https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2016.00069 Lamy, D., Carmel, T.,Egeth, H. E., & Leber, A. B. (2006). Effects of search mode and intertrial priming on singleton search. Perception and Psychophysics, 68(6), 919-932. Lamy, D., & Egeth, H. E. (2003). Attentional capture in singleton-detection and feature-search modes. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Human Perception and Performance, 29(5), 1003-1020. Li, L., Gratton, C.,Fabiani, M., & Knight, R. T. (2013). Age-related frontoparietal changes during the control of bottom-up and top-down attention:An ERP study. Neurobiology of Aging, 34(2), 477-488. Lien, M.,Gemperle, A., & Ruthruff, E. (2011). Aging and involuntary attention capture:Electrophysiological evidence for preserved attentional control with advanced age. Psychology and Aging, 26(1), 188-203. Li, S. C., & Lindenberger, U. (1999). Cross-level unification:a computational exploration of the link between deterioration of neurotransmitter systems and dedifferentiation of cognitive abilities in old age. In L. G. Nilsson, & H. Markowitsch (Eds.), Cognitive neuroscience of memory (pp.103-146). Hogrefe and Huber. Madden, D. J., Parks, E. L., Davis, S. W., Diaz, M. T., Potter, G. G., Chou, Y. H., … Cabeza, R. (2014). Age mediation of frontoparietal activation during visual feature search.NeuroImage, 102(P2), 262-274. Manard, M., Franc, S., Phillips, C., & Salmon, E. (2017). The neural bases of proactive and reactive control processes in normal aging. Behavioural Brain Research, 320, 504-516. Mattson, M. P., Duan, W., Chan, S. L., Cheng, A.,Haughey, N., Gary, D. S., … Furukawa, K. (2002). Neuroprotective and neurorestorative signal transduction mechanisms in brain aging:Modification by genes, diet and behavior. Neurobiology of Aging, 23(5), 695-705. Mertes, C., Wascher, E., & Schneider, D. (2017). Compliance instead of flexibility? On age-related differences in cognitive control during visual search. Neurobiology of Aging, 53, 169-180. Monge, Z. A.,Geib, B. R., Siciliano, R. E., Packard, L. E., Tallman, W., & Madden, D. J. (2017). Functional modular architecture underlying attentional control in aging. NeuroImage, 155, 257-270. Nasreddine, Z. S., Phillips, N. A., Charbonneau, S., Whitehead, V., Collin, I., Cummings, J. L., & Chertkow, H. (2005). The montreal cognitive assessment, MoCA:A brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 53(4), 695-699. O'Sullivan, M., Jones, D. K., Summers, P. E., Morris, R. G., Williams, S. C. R., & Markus, H. S. (2001). Evidence for cortical "disconnection" as a mechanism of age-related cognitive decline.Neurology, 57(4), 632-638. Phillips, S., & Takeda, Y. (2010). Frontal-parietal synchrony in elderly EEG for visual search.International Journal of Psychophysiology, 75(1), 39-43. Reuter-lorenz, P. A., & Cappell, K. A. (2008). Neurocognitive aging and the compensation hypothesis. Association for Psychological Science, 17(3), 177-182. Salthouse, T. A. (1996). The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition.Psychlolgical Review, 103(3), 403-428. Spreng, R. N., Stevens, W. D., Viviano, J. D., & Schacter, D. L. (2016). Attenuated anticorrelation between the default and dorsal attention networks with aging:Evidence from task and rest. Neurobiology of Aging, 45, 149-160. Theeuwes, J. (1991). Exogenous and endogenous control of attention:The effect of visual onsets and offsets. Perception & Psychophysics, 49(1), 83-90. Theeuwes, J. (1992). Perceptual selectivity for color and form. Perception & Psychophysics, 51(6), 599-606. Theeuwes, J. (2004). Top-down search strategies cannot override attentional capture. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11(1), 65-70. Theeuwes, J. (2010). Top-down and bottom-up control of visual selection. Acta Psychologica, 135(2), 77-99. Tsvetanov, K. A.,Mevorach, C., Allen, H., & Humphreys, G. W. (2013). Age-related differences in selection by visual saliency. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 75(7), 1382-1394. Veen, V. V., & Carter, C. S. (2002). The anterior cingulate as a conflict monitor:fMRI and ERP studies. Physiology & Behavior, 77(4-5), 477-482. Wiegand, I., Finke, K., Müller, H. J., & Töllner, T. (2013). Event-related potentials dissociate perceptual from response-related age effects in visual search. Neurobiology of Aging, 34(3), 973-985. 刘丽, 白学军. (2016). 注意控制定势和线索类型在注意捕获中的作用. 心理学报, 48(9), 1093-1104. 王炜, 刘丹丹, 高中宝, 解恒革, 周波, 陈彤, 张晓红. (2010). 蒙特利尔认知评估量表(中文版)在驻京军队离退休干部中界值划分的初步研究. 中华保健医学杂志, 12(4), 271-273. |
[1] | 孙赛男, 何文广. 语音加工的发展、老化及其神经机制[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2022, 38(6): 894-901. |
[2] | 潘毅, 张琳. 工作记忆内容驱动完全无关刺激捕获注意[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2019, 35(5): 522-529. |
[3] | 宋晓蕾, 李小芳, 赵媛, 何丹. 不同任务难度条件下认知老化对冲突适应能力的影响[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2017, 33(5): 569-576. |
[4] | 濮冰燕, 彭华茂. 认知老化对于老年人决策过程的影响:动机的调节作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2016, 32(1): 114-120. |
[5] | 葛国宏, 金一波. 非期望刺激的生态意义信息对无意视盲的影响:情绪效价和精细程度的作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2015, 31(6): 661-667. |
[6] | 高悦, 彭华茂, 文静, 王大华. 教育水平在认知老化中的作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2011, 27(6): 591-598. |
|