心理发展与教育 ›› 2013, Vol. 29 ›› Issue (6): 571-577.

• 认知与社会性发展 • 上一篇    下一篇

自闭症儿童视觉动态信息的注意加工特点——来自多目标追踪任务的证据

马玉1,2, 张学民1,3, 张盈利1,5, 魏柳青4   

  1. 1. 北京师范大学心理学院, 北京 100875;
    2. 宁夏工商职业技术学院思想政治教学部, 银川 750021;
    3. 北京师范大学认知神经科学与学习国家重点实验室, 北京 100875;
    4. 河南省体育科学研究所, 郑州 450044;
    5. 珠海市卫生学校, 珠海 519020
  • 出版日期:2013-11-15 发布日期:2013-11-15
  • 通讯作者: 张学民,E-mail:xmzhang@bnu.edu.cn E-mail:xmzhang@bnu.edu.cn
  • 基金资助:
    国家自然科学基金项目“多目标视觉追踪中注意分配的认知神经机制研究”(31271083);国家重点基础研究计划(973项目)(2011CB711000).

Attention Processing Characteristics of Children with Autism:Evidence from the Multiple Objects Tracking Task

MA Yu1,2, ZHANG Xue-min1,3, ZHANG Ying-li1,5, WEI Liu-qing4   

  1. 1. Beijing Normal University, School of Psychology, Beijing 100875, China;
    2. Ideological and Political Education Department, Ningxia Business Technology Institute, Yinchuan 750021, China;
    3. State Key Lab of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning, Beijing 100875, China;
    4. Henan Institute of Sport Science, Zhengzhou 450044, China;
    5. Zhuhai Medical Education School, Zhuhai 519020, China
  • Online:2013-11-15 Published:2013-11-15

摘要: 为了考察中低功能自闭症儿童在动态条件下的多目标注意加工特点,研究采用多目标追踪实验范式,具体追踪任务采用多目标随机运动和碰撞反弹算法。实验设计为两因素混合设计,组间变量为年龄和性别匹配的中低功能自闭症和正常儿童组,组内变量为追踪目标数量(分别为1、2、3、4个目标),因变量为被试在多目标追踪任务中的追踪正确率和追踪容量。结果发现:(1)中低功能自闭症儿童在目标数量为1到目标数量为4时的正确率均明显低于正常儿童,且标准差变异较大,中低功能自闭症儿童的多目标的持续追踪能力和稳定性存在一定的缺陷;(2)当目标数量为1时,正常组儿童与中低功能自闭症儿童的追踪容量差异不显著;目标为2、3、4时,两组追踪容量差异均显著。不同目标数量情况下中低功能自闭症儿童平均的注意容量为0.83~1.24个(平均容量约为1个)明显低于正常儿童的0.95~3.72个(最大容量接近4个)。中低功能自闭症儿童在多目标追踪上表现为单焦点注意加工的特点,存在一定程度的注意转移缺陷。

关键词: 中低功能自闭症儿童, 多目标追踪, 视觉注意特点

Abstract: The present study was intended to investigate the visual attention processing characteristics of children with autism using dynamic visual tracking task. In present study, the typical multiple objects tracking paradigm was adopted,both age and gender matched mid-low Functioning autistic children and normal control group children participated in the same tracking task。There were eight objects in the tracking task,which one, two, three, or four objects were targets and the others were non targets (distractors), the multiple objects tracking accuracy was recorded, and tracking capacity was calculated. The results showed that: (1) Tracking accuracy of autism was significantly lower than the normal control group children from one target to four targets conditions, which indicated autism group had low performance on tracking stability and persistence; (2) when the target number was one, there was no significant difference on the tracking capacity between normal control group children and autistic children; when the number of targets were two, three, and four, the tracking capacity of normal group of children were high than the autistic children significantly. This indicated that autism group was one focus attentional tracking mode and low performance on attentional shift.

Key words: Mid-low Functioning Autistic Children, Multiple Object Tracking, Characteristics of Visual Attention

中图分类号: 

  • B844.1
Ashida, H., Seiffert, A. E., & Osaka, N. (2001). Inefficient visual search for second-order motion. JOSA A, 18(9), 2255-2266.
Benson, V., Piper, J., & Fletcher-Watson, S. (2009). Atypical saccadic scanning in autistic spectrum disorder. Neuropsychologia, 47(4), 1178-1182.
Bertone, A., Mottron, L., Jelenic, P., & Faubert, J. (2003). Motion perception in autism: a complex issue. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(2), 218-225.
Dawson, G., Meltzoff, A. N., Osterling, J., Rinaldi, J., & Brown, E. (1998). Children with autism fail to orient to naturally occurring social stimuli. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 28(6), 479-485.
Dawson, G., Webb, S. J., & McPartland, J. (2005). Understanding the nature of face processing impairment in autism: insights from behavioral and electrophysiological studies. Developmental neuropsychology, 27(3), 403-424.
Drew, T., McCollough, A. W., Horowitz, T. S., & Vogel, E. K. (2009). Attentional enhancement during multiple-object tracking. Psychonomic bulletin & review, 16(2), 411-417.
Eagle, R. S. (2002). Accessing and assessing intelligence in individuals with lower functioning autism. Journal of Developmental Disabilities, 9, 45-53.
Falck-Ytter, T., Fernell, E., Hedvall, A. L., von Hofsten, C., & Gillberg, C. (2012). Gaze performance in children with autism spectrum disorder when observing communicative actions. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 42(10), 2236-2245.
Fletcher-Watson, S., Leekam, S. R., Benson, V., Frank, M. C., & Findlay, J. M. (2009). Eye-movements reveal attention to social information in autism spectrum disorder. Neuropsychologia, 47(1), 248-257.
Fombonne, & Eric.(1999). The epidemiology of autism: a review. Psychological medicine, 4(29), 769-786.
Happé, F. (1999). Autism: cognitive deficit or cognitive style? Trends in cognitive sciences, 3(6), 216-222.
Hill, E. L. (2004). Evaluating the theory of executive dysfunction in autism. Developmental Review, 24(2), 189-233.
Horowitz, T. S., Klieger, S. B., Fencsik, D. E., Yang, K. K., Alvarez, G. A., & Wolfe, J. M. (2007). Tracking unique objects. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 69(2), 172-184.
Iarocci, G., & Burack, J. A. (2004). Intact covert orienting to peripheral cues among children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(3), 257-264.
Johnson, M. H., Griffin, R., Csibra, G., Halit, H., Farroni, T., De Haan, M., et al. (2005). The emergence of the social brain network: Evidence from typical and atypical development. Development and psychopathology, 17(3), 599-619.
Jones, C. R., Swettenham, J., Charman, T., Marsden, A. J., Tregay, J., Baird, G., et al. (2011). No evidence for a fundamental visual motion processing deficit in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Autism Research, 4(5), 347-357.
Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort Engwood cliff, N.J.:Prentice-Hall.
Kikuchi, Y., Senju, A., Akechi, H., Tojo, Y., Osanai, H., & Hasegawa, T. (2011). Atypical disengagement from faces and its modulation by the control of eye fixation in children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 41(5), 629-645.
Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F., & Cohen, D. (2002). Defining and quantifying the social phenotype in autism. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159(6), 895.
Koldewyn, K., Weigelt, S., Kanwisher, N., & Jiang, Y. (2013). Multiple object tracking in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 43(6), 1394-1405.
Kylli Inen, A., & Hietanen, J. K. (2004). Attention orienting by another's gaze direction in children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(3), 435-444.
Maekawa, T., Tobimatsu, S., Inada, N., Oribe, N., Onitsuka, T., Kanba, S., et al. (2011). Top-down and bottom-up visual information processing of non-social stimuli in high-functioning autism spectrum disorder. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5(1), 201-209.
Milne, E., Swettenham, J., Hansen, P., Campbell, R., Jeffries, H., & Plaisted, K. (2002). High motion coherence thresholds in children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43(2), 255-263.
Pylyshyn, Z. W., & Storm, R. W. (1988). Tracking multiple independent targets: Evidence for a parallel tracking mechanism. Spatial vision, 3(3), 179-197.
Rutherford, M. D., Richards, E. D., Moldes, V., & Sekuler, A. B. (2007). Evidence of a divided-attention advantage in autism. Cognitive neuropsychology, 24(5), 505-515.
Senju, A., Tojo, Y., Yaguchi, K., & Hasegawa, T. (2005). Deviant gaze processing in children with autism: an ERP study. Neuropsychologia, 43(9), 1297-1306.
Trick, L. M., Perl, T., & Sethi, N. (2005). Age-related differences in multiple-object tracking. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 60(2), 102.
Trick, L., Hollinsworth, H., & Brodeur, D. A. (2009). 3 Multiple-object tracking across the lifespan: do different factors contribute to diminished performance in different age groups? Computation, cognition, and Pylyshyn, 79.
Van der Geest, J. N., Kemner, C., Verbaten, M. N., & Van Engeland, H. (2002). Gaze behavior of children with pervasive developmental disorder toward human faces: a fixation time study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43(5), 669-678.
ZHang, X., Yan, M., & Liao, Y. (2009). Differential performance of chinese volleyball athletes and nonathletes on a multiple-object tracking task 1, 2.Perceptual and Motor Skills, 109(3), 747-756.
中华医学会精神病学分会. (2001). 中国精神障碍分类与诊断标准 第三版. 山东科学出版社.
[1] 苏晶, 段东园, 张学民. 负性情绪刺激对大学生多目标追踪能力的影响[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2016, 32(5): 521-531.
Viewed
Full text


Abstract

Cited

  Shared   
  Discussed   
No Suggested Reading articles found!