心理发展与教育 ›› 2024, Vol. 40 ›› Issue (6): 761-773.doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2024.06.01
• 认知与社会性发展 •
梁渊1,2, 张丽锦2,3,4, 冀婷1, 李博艺1, 卜凡1
LIANG Yuan1,2, ZHANG Lijin2,3,4, JI Ting1, LI Boyi1, BU Fan1
摘要: 以“自动关注数量信息(SFON)”倾向为指标考查4、5岁幼儿的数量敏锐度,探讨不同数量敏锐度水平的幼儿在三种形式的数轴估计(非符号面积、数字-位置(NP)和位置-数字(PN))任务上的表现及策略使用情况。结果发现:(1)4岁幼儿敏锐度可分为次高、中、低三个水平,5岁幼儿敏锐度可分为高、中两个水平,幼儿捕捉数量信息的敏锐度水平从4岁到5岁明显提升,由依赖参考提示逐渐转变为独立自发感知;(2)幼儿数轴估计表现受情境的影响,在非符号数轴估计任务上的表现显著优于在符号NP和PN任务上的表现;(3)数量敏锐度在一定程度上影响数轴估计表现及策略使用,高数量敏锐度幼儿的数轴估计成绩更好,大多数5岁幼儿基本可以在不同形式的数轴估计任务上灵活地选用合适的数轴估计策略。本研究结果揭示了幼儿心理数量表征的内部机制,证实了数量认知的四步发展模型,为鉴别、预测和筛选出低敏锐度的数学风险幼儿提供参考依据。
中图分类号:
Ashcraft, M. H., & Moore, A. M. (2012). Cognitive processes of numerical estimation in children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 111(2), 246-267. Barth, H. C., & Paladino, A. M. (2011). The development of numerical estimation: Evidence against a representational shift. Developmental Science, 14(1), 125-135. Batchelor, S., Inglis, M., & Gilmore, C. (2015). Spontaneous focusing on numerosity and the arithmetic advantage. Learning and Instruction, 40(2-3), 116-135. Bojorque, G., Torbeyns, J., Hannula-Sormunen, M., Van Nijlen, D., & Verschaffel, L. (2017). Development of SFON in Ecuadorian kindergartners. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 32, 449-462. Cantlon, J. F., Safford, K. E., & Brannon, E. M. (2010). Spontaneous analog number representations in 3-year-old children. Developmental Science, 13(2), 289-297. Chan, J. Y., & Mazzocco, M. M. (2017). Competing features influence children's attention to number. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 156, 62-81. Chen, C., & Stevenson, H. W. (1988). Cross-linguistic differences in digit span of preschool children.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 46(1), 150-158. Cicchini, G. M., Anobile, G., & Burr, D. C. (2014). Compressive mapping of number to space reflects dynamic encoding mechanisms, not static logarithmic transform.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(21), 7867-7872. Cicchini, G. M., Anobile, G., & Burr, D. C. (2019). Spontaneous representation of numerosity in typical and dyscalculic development. Cortex, 114, 151-163. Deng, C., Silinskas, G., Wei, W., & Georgiou, G. (2015). Cross-lagged relationships between home literacy/numeracy environment and academic achievement in Chinese.Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 33, 12-20. Ebersbach, M. (2016). Development of children's estimation skills: The ambiguous role of their familiarity with numerals. Child Development Perspectives, 10(2), 116-121. Ebersbach, M., Luwel, K., Frick, A., Onghena, P., & Verschaffel, L. (2008).The relationship between the shape of the mental number line and familiarity with numbers in 5- to 9-year old children: Evidence for a segmented linear model. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 99(1), 1-17. Fuhs, M. W., Nesbitt, K. T., & Connor, D. O. (2018). Approximate number system task performance: Associations with domain-general and domain-specific cognitive skills in young children.Journal of Numerical Cognition, 4(3), 590-612. Gelman, R. (2009). Learning in core and noncore domains. In L. Tommasi, M. A. Peterson, & L. Nadal (Eds.),Cognitive biology: Evolutionary and developmental perspectives on mind, brain, and behavior (pp. 247-260). MIT Press. Gelman, R., & Williams, E. M. (1998). Enabling constraints for cognitive development and learning: Domain specificity and epigenesis. In W. Damon, D. Kuhn, & R. S. Siegler (Eds.).Handbook of child psychology: Cognition, perception, and language (5th ed., Vol. 1, pp. 575-630). Wiley. Gray, S. A., & Reeve, R. A. (2016). Number-specific and general cognitive markers of preschoolers’ math ability profiles. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 147, 1-21. Hannula, M. M., & Lehtinen, E. (2005). Spontaneous focusing on numerosity and mathematical skills of young children. Learning and Instruction, 15(3), 237-256. Hannula-Sormunen, M. M., Lehtinen, E., & Räsänen, P. (2015). Preschool children's spontaneous focusing on numerosity, subitizing and counting skills as predictors of their mathematical performance 7 years later at school.Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 17(2-3), 155-177. Hannula, M. M., Lepola, J., & Lehtinen, E. (2010). Spontaneous focusing on numerosity as a domain-specific predictor of arithmetical skills. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 107(4), 394-406. Hannula-Sormunen, M.(2014). Spontaneous focusing on numerosity and its relation to counting and arithmetic. In R. Cohen-Kadosh & A. Dowker (Eds.), Handbook of Numerical Cognition (pp.275-290). Oxford University Press. Hollands, J. G. & Dyre, B. P. (2000). Bias in proportion judgments:The cyclical power model. Psychological Review, 107(3), 500-524. Jung, T., & Wickrama, K. (2008). An introduction to latent class growth analysis and growth mixture modeling.Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2(1), 302-317. Kucian, K., Ashkenazi, S. S., Hänggi, J., Rotzer, S., Jäncke, L., Martin, E., & von Aster, M. (2014). Developmental dyscalculia: A dysconnection syndrome? Brain Structure and Function, 219(5), 1721-1733. Lanza, S. T., Collins, J. L., Lemmon, D., & Schafer, J. L. (2007). PROC LCA: A SAS procedure for latent class analysis.Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 14(4), 671-694. Lehtinen, E., Hannula-Sormunen, M., McMullen, J., & Gruber, H. (2017). Cultivating mathematical skills: From drill-and-practice to deliberate practice.ZDM Mathematics Education, 49, 625-636. Liang Y., Zhang, L., Long, Y., Deng, Q., & Liu, Y. (2020). Promoting effects of RTI-based mathematical play training on number sense growth among low-SES preschool children. Early Education and Development, 31(3), 335-353. Magidson, J., & Vermunt, J. K. (2003). Comparing latent class factor analysis with traditional approach in data mining. In H. Bozdogan (Ed.),Statistical data mining and knowledge discovery (pp. 373-383). Chapman and Hall/CRC Press. McMullen, J., Chan, J. Y.-C., Mazzocco, M. M. M., & Hannula-Sormunen, M. M. (2019). Spontaneous mathematical focusing tendencies in mathematical development and education. In A. Norton & M. W. Alibali (Eds.), Constructing number: Merging perspectives from psychology and mathematics education (pp. 69-86). Springer International Publishing. Miller, K., Major, S. M., Shu, H., & Zhang, H. (2000). Ordinal knowledge: Number names and number concepts in Chinese and English.Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54, 129-140. Nanu, C. E., McMullen, J., Munck, P., & Hannula-Sormunen, M. M. (2018). Spontaneous focusing on numerosity in preschool as a predictor of mathematical skills and knowledge in the fifth grade. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 169, 42-58. Nylund, K. L., Asparouhov, T., & Muthén, B. O. (2007). Deciding on the number of classes in latent class analysis and growth mixture modeling: A Monte Carlo simulation study.Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 14(4), 535-569. Odic, D., Libertus, M. E., Feigenson, L., & Halberda, J. (2013). Developmental change in the acuity of approximate number and area representations. Developmental Psychology, 49(6), 1103-1112. Opfer, J. E., Siegler, R. S., & Young, C. J. (2011). The powers of noise-fitting: Reply to Barth and Paladino. Developmental Science, 14(5), 1194-1204. Peeters, D., Degrande, T., Ebersbach, M., Verschaffel, L., & Luwel, K. (2016). Children’s use of number line estimation strategies.European Journal of Psychology of Education, 31(2), 117-134. Rathé, S., Torbeyns, J., Hannula-Sormunen, M. M., De Smedt, B., & Verschaffel, L. (2016). Spontaneous focusing on numerosity: A review of recent research.Mediterrean Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 15, 1-25. Rathé, S., Torbeyns, J., De Smedt, B., & Verschaffel, L. (2019). Spontaneous focusing on Arabic number symbols and its association with early mathematical competencies.Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 48, 111-121. Rathé, S., Torbeyns, J., De Smedt, B., & Verschaffel, L. (2022). Longitudinal associations between spontaneous number focusing tendencies, numerical abilities, and mathematics achievement in 4- to 7-year-olds. Journal of Educational Psychology, 114(1), 37-55. Reeve, R., Reynolds, F., Humberstone, J., & Butterworth, B. (2012). Stability and change in markers of core numerical competencies. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology: General, 141(4), 649-666. Schneider, M., Merz, S., Stricker, J., De Smedt, B., Torbeyns, J., Verschaffel, L., & Luwel, K. (2018). Associations of number line estimation with mathematical competence: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 89(5), 1467-1484. Sella, F., Berteletti, I., Lucangeli, D., & Zorzi, M. (2015). Varieties of quantity estimation in children. Developmental Psychology, 51(6), 758-770. Siegler, R. S., & Opfer, J. E. (2003). The development of numerical estimation: Evidence for multiple representations of numerical quantity.Psychological Science, 14(3), 237-250. Siegler, R. S., Thompson, C. A., & Opfer, J. E. (2010). The logarithmic-to-linear shift: One learning sequence, many tasks, many time scales. Mind Brain & Education, 3(3), 143-150. Slusser, E. B., Santiago, R. T., & Barth, H. C. (2013). Developmental change in numerical estimation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(1), 193-200. Slusser, E., & Barth, H. (2011).An Excel worksheet for proportion-judgment analyses of number-line data. Department of Psychology, Wesleyan University, Middletown, CT. Hilary Barth. https://hbarth.faculty.wesleyan.edu Slusser, E., & Barth, H. (2017). Intuitive proportion judgment in number-line estimation: Converging evidence from multiple tasks. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 162, 181-198. Spelke, E. S. (2017). Core knowledge, language, and number.Language Learning and Development, 13(2), 147-170. Stoianov, I. P., & Zorzi, M. (2012). The abstraction of visual numerosity from continous visual properties. Perception ECVP Abstract, 41, 256. Torbeyns, J., Bojorque, G., Van Hoof, J., Van Nijlen, D., & Verschaffel, L. (2018). Unique contribution of Ecuadorian kindergartners' spontaneous focusing on numerosity to their early numerical abilities.British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 36(2), 299-312. Vermunt, J. K., & Magidson, J. (2013). Technical guide for Latent GOLD 5.0: Basic, advanced, and syntax. Statistical Innovations. Verschaffel, L., Rathé, S., Wijns, N., Degrande, T., Van Dooren, W., De Smedt, B., & Torbeyns, J. (2020). Young children's early mathematical competencies: The role of mathematical focusing tendencies. In M. Carlsen, I. Erfjord & P. Hundeland (Eds.),Mathematics education in the early years (pp. 23-42). Springer-Cham. von Aster, R. S., & Shalev, S. S. (2007). Number development and developmental dyscalculia.Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 49(11), 868-873. Wong, T. T.-Y., & Chan, W. W. L. (2019). Identifying children with persistent low math achievement: The role of number-magnitude mapping and symbolic numerical processing.Learning and Instruction, 60, 29-40. 崔爽, 高亚茹, 王阳阳, 黄碧娟, 司继伟. (2020). 非正式学习环境中幼儿的自发数量聚焦. 心理科学进展, 28(12), 2064-2075. 张洁婷, 焦璨, 张敏强. (2010). 潜在类别分析技术在心理学研究中的应用. 心理科学进展, 18(12), 1991-1998. |
[1] | 朱晶晶, 张珍珍, 李燕. 母亲抑郁与幼儿社会适应: 母亲的悲伤反应与亲子冲突的中介作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(5): 675-686. |
[2] | 刘建榕, 陈晴昕, 童宁. 3~4岁幼儿心理理论的微观发生研究[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(3): 305-315. |
[3] | 张文洁, 黄子君, 谭佩霞, 范伟, 钟毅平. 为了谁,幼儿更可能说谎?不同受益者对5~6岁幼儿说谎行为的影响[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2024, 40(3): 316-323. |
[4] | 高子惠, 焦雨, 王曦, 刘肖岑. 电子绘本文字的动静态呈现方式对幼儿阅读体验和学习效果的影响[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(6): 817-824. |
[5] | 王英杰, 栾金鑫, 李燕. 母亲养育压力与幼儿焦虑的关系:亲子冲突的中介作用和母亲正念的调节作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(5): 702-709. |
[6] | 张文洁, 范增, 黎蕾, 刘毅, 钟毅平. 接受对象的常规行为与资源需求对5~6岁幼儿分享行为的影响[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(4): 465-472. |
[7] | 牛湘, 冉光明. 同伴关系与幼儿问题行为关系的三水平元分析[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(4): 473-487. |
[8] | 刘双, 李梅, 冯晓杭, 张向葵. 幼儿掌控动机与能力关系的纵向研究:任务类型与气质的调节作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(2): 161-172. |
[9] | 王英杰, 张刈, 张美霞, 李燕. 母亲正念与幼儿问题行为的关系:正念养育和亲子关系的链式中介作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(2): 184-191. |
[10] | 刘晓洁, 李燕. 母亲感知的共同教养对幼儿行为问题的影响:一个有调节的中介模型[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2022, 38(5): 626-634. |
[11] | 张和颐, 洪秀敏. 家庭认知环境与0~3岁婴幼儿发展的关系:有调节的中介效应[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2022, 38(2): 178-185. |
[12] | 王薇, 贡德英, 程春, 陈科, 李永鑫, 闫晶晶. 5~7岁自闭症谱系障碍幼儿数感的特点[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2021, 37(6): 818-825. |
[13] | 刘田田, 李燕, 李有嘉, 姜新苗. 父母协同教养与学前儿童社会行为的关系:亲子关系和同胞关系的链式中介作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2021, 37(5): 638-647. |
[14] | 周思妤, 王艺卓, 李晓巍. 幼儿园新教师学历与离职意向的关系:入职适应、工作满意度的多重中介作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2021, 37(5): 675-682. |
[15] | 贺晓玲, 陈俊, 刘灵, 张甜. 3~5岁幼儿职业声望垂直空间、大小双重隐喻发展[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2021, 37(4): 457-464. |
|