心理发展与教育 ›› 2024, Vol. 40 ›› Issue (3): 316-323.doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2024.03.02

• 认知与社会性发展 • 上一篇    下一篇

为了谁,幼儿更可能说谎?不同受益者对5~6岁幼儿说谎行为的影响

张文洁2,3, 黄子君1,2, 谭佩霞2,3, 范伟1,2, 钟毅平1,2   

  1. 1. 湖南师范大学教育科学学院心理系, 长沙 410081;
    2. 认知与人类行为湖南省重点实验室, 长沙 410081;
    3. 湖南师范大学教育科学学院学前教育系, 长沙 410081
  • 发布日期:2024-05-15
  • 通讯作者: 钟毅平 E-mail:ypzhong@hunnu.edu.cn
  • 基金资助:
    国家社会科学基金教育学青年课题“幼儿分享行为的心理机制:接受者的自我相关性与需要的作用”(CBA190244)。

Who Are Young Children More Likely to Lie for? The Influence of Different Beneficiaries on Lying Behavior among Preschoolers Aged 5~6 Years

ZHANG Wenjie2,3, HUANG Zijun1,2, TAN Peixia2,3, FAN Wei1,2, ZHONG Yiping1,2   

  1. 1. Department of Psychology, School of Educational Science, Hunan Normal University, Changsha 410081;
    2. Cognition and Human Behavior Key Laboratory of Hunan Province, Changsha 410081;
    3. Department of Preschool Education, School of Educational Science, Hunan Normal University, Changsha 410081
  • Published:2024-05-15

摘要: 本研究采用“你藏我找”范式,将自我受益作为基线条件,通过两个实验分别考察不同自我相关性受益者和不同群体受益者对5~6岁幼儿说谎行为的影响。结果发现:(1)相比于高自我相关者和低自我相关者受益条件,幼儿更可能为自己的利益而说谎;而相比于低自我相关者受益条件,幼儿更可能为高自我相关者的利益而说谎(实验1);(2)相比于内群体和外群体受益条件,幼儿更可能为自己的利益而说谎;而相比于外群体受益条件,幼儿更可能为内群体的利益而说谎(实验2)。以上结果表明,5~6岁幼儿的说谎行为存在强烈的利己倾向,为自己获得利益而说谎的可能性最大;而5~6岁幼儿的亲社会说谎行为在个体层面存在自我相关性偏好,在群体层面存在内群体偏好。

关键词: 5~6岁幼儿说谎行为, 受益者, 利己倾向, 自我相关性偏好, 内群体偏好

Abstract: Experiment 1 adopted the “Hide-and-Seek Game task” to examine the influence of beneficiaries with different extents of self-relevance on lying behavior among preschoolers aged 5~6 years. The results of Experiment 1 suggested that participants told more lies for their own benefits compared with the benefits of the highly self-relevant beneficiary (friends) and lowly self-relevant beneficiary (strangers). Nevertheless, compared with the benefits of the lowly self-relevant beneficiary (strangers), participants were more likely to lie for the benefits of the highly self-relevant beneficiary (friends). Experiment 2 also used the “Hide-and-Seek Game task” to investigate the influence of beneficiaries of different groups on lying behavior among preschoolers aged 5~6 years. The results of Experiment 2 also found that participants were more likely to lie for their own benefits compared with the benefits of the in-group beneficiary and out-group beneficiary. In addition, by comparison with the benefits of the out-group beneficiary, participants were more likely to lie for the benefits of the in-group beneficiary. Our findings indicate that there is a strong egoistic tendency in lying behavior among preschoolers aged 5~6 years. More importantly, prosocial lying behavior among preschoolers aged 5~6 years is affected by self-relevance at the individual level, and there is “Self-relevance preference”. At the group level, prosocial lying behavior among preschoolers aged 5~6 years is influenced by group differences, and there is “In-group favoritism”.

Key words: lying behavior of preschoolers aged 5~6 years, beneficiaries, egoistic tendency, self-relevance preference, in-group favoritism

中图分类号: 

  • B844
Brewer, M. B., & Yuki, M. (2007). Culture and social identity. In S. Kitayama & D. Cohen (Eds.),Handbook of cultural psychology (pp. 307-322). The Guilford Press.
Brown, A. L., Meer, J., & Williams, J. F. (2017). Social distance and quality ratings in charity choice.Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Economics, 66, 9-15.
Buhs, E. S., Ladd, G. W., & Herald, S. L. (2006). Peer exclusion and victimization: Processes that mediate the relation between peer group rejection and children’s classroom engagement and achievement? Journal of Educational Psychology, 98(1), 1-13.
Cialdini, R. B., Brown, S. L., Lewis, B. P., Luce, C., & Neuberg, S. L. (1997). Reinterpreting the empathy-altruism relationship: When one into one equals oneness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(3), 481-494.
Darwin, C. (1877). A biographical sketch of an infant.Mind, 2(7), 285-294.
De La Cerda, C., & Warnell, K. R. (2020). Young children’s willingness to deceive shows in-group bias only in specific social contexts. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 198, 104906.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2020.104906
Demedardi, M. J., Brechet, C., Gentaz, E., & Monnier, C. (2021). Prosocial lying in children between 4 and 11 years of age: The role of emotional understanding and empathy. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 203, 105045.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2020.105045
Dubow, E. F., & Cappas, C. L. (1988). Peer social status and reports of children’s adjustment by their teachers, by their peers, and by their self-ratings. Journal of School Psychology, 26(1), 69-75.
Dunham, Y., Baron, A. S., & Carey, S. (2011). Consequences of “minimal” group affiliations in children.Child Development, 82(3), 793-811.
Erat, S., & Gneezy, U. (2012). White lies.Management Science, 58(4), 723-733.
Everett, J. A., Faber, N. S., & Crockett, M. J. (2015). The influence of social preferences and reputational concerns on intergroup prosocial behaviour in gains and losses contexts.Royal Society Open Science, 2(12), 150546.https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.150546
Fan, W., Chen, J., Wang, X. Y., Cai, R., Tan, Q., Chen, Y., … Zhong, Y. (2013). Electrophysiological correlation of the degree ofself-reference effect. PLoS ONE, 8(12), e80289.https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0080289
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences.Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175-191.
Fu, G., Xu, F., Cameron, C. A., Heyman, G., & Lee, K. (2007). Cross-cultural differences in children’s choices, categorizations, and evaluations of truths and lies. Developmental Psychology, 43(2), 278-293.
Hala, S., & Russell, J. (2001). Executive control within strategic deception: A window on early cognitive development?Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 80(2), 112-141.
Heinrich, M., & Liszkowski, U. (2021). Three-year-olds’ spontaneous lying in a novel interaction-based paradigm and its relations to explicit skills and motivational factors. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 207, 105125.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2021.105125
Johnson, C., Gadon, O., Carlson, D., Southwick, S., Faith, M., & Chalfin, J. (2002). Self-reference and group membership: Evidence for a group-reference effect.European Journal of Social Psychology, 32(2), 261-274.
Kim, H., Schnall, S., Yi, D. J., & White, M. P. (2013). Social distance decreases responders’ sensitivity to fairness in the ultimatum game. Judgment and Decision Making, 8(5), 632-638.
Leduc, K., Williams, S., Gomez‐Garibello, C., & Talwar, V. (2017). The contributions of mental state understanding and executive functioning to preschool-aged children’s lie-telling. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 35(2), 288-302.
Lee, K. (2013). Little liars: Development of verbal deception in children.Child Development Perspectives, 7(2), 91-96.
Lewis, M., Stanger, C., & Sullivan, M. W. (1989). Deception in 3-year-olds.Developmental Psychology, 25(3), 439-443.
Liang, G., Fan, W., Hu, Z., Zhang, W., & Zhong, Y. (2022). More intimate, more generous? Behaviour control can reduce children’s self-relevance effect in sharing behaviour. Early Child Development and Care, 192(1), 24-35.
Liberman, N., Trope, Y., & Stephan, E. (2007). Psychological distance. In A. W. Kruglanski & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 353-381). The Guilford Press.
Moore, C. (2009). Fairness in children’s resource allocation depends on the recipient. Psychological Science, 20(8), 944-948.
Olson, K. R., & Spelke, E. S. (2008). Foundations of cooperation in young children.Cognition, 108(1), 222-231.
Paulus, M. (2015). Children’s inequity aversion depends on culture: A cross-cultural comparison.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 132, 240-246.
Paulus, M., & Moore, C. (2014). The development of recipient-dependent sharing behavior and sharing expectations in preschool children.Developmental Psychology, 50(3), 914-921.
Piaget, J. (1932).The moral judgment of the child. New York: Free Press.
Ratner, R. K., & Miller, D. T. (2001). The norm of self-interest and its effects on social action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(1), 5-16.
Russell, J., Mauthner, N., Sharpe, S., & Tidswell, T. (1991). The 'windows task’ as a measure of strategic deception in preschoolers and autistic subjects.British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9(2), 331-349.
Sabato, H., & Kogut, T. (2021). Sharing and belonging: Children’s social status and their sharing behavior with in-group and out-group members. Developmental Psychology, 57(12), 2082-2092.
Scharpf, F., Paulus, M., & Wörle, M. (2017). The impact of social relationships on Ugandan children’s sharing decisions.European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 14(4), 436-448.
Schreuders, E., Klapwijk, E. T., Will, G. J., & Güroglu, B. (2018). Friend versus foe: Neural correlates of prosocial decisions for liked and disliked peers.Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 18(1), 127-142.
Sierksma, J., Spaltman, M., & Lansu, T. A. (2019). Children tell more prosocial lies in favor of in-group than out-group peers.Developmental Psychology, 55(7), 1428-1439.
Sierksma, J., Thijs, J., & Verkuyten, M. (2014). Children’s intergroup helping: The role of empathy and peer group norms. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 126, 369-383.
Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (2004). The social identity theory of intergroup behavior. In J. T. Jost & J. Sidanius (Eds.),Political psychology: Key readings (pp. 276-293). Psychology Press.
Talwar, V., Lee, K., Bala, N., & Lindsay, R. C. L. (2002). Children’s conceptual knowledge of lying and its relation to their actual behaviors: Implications for court competence examinations. Law and Human Behavior, 26(4), 395-415.
Talwar, V., Murphy, S. M., & Lee, K. (2007). White lie-telling in children for politeness purposes. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 31(1), 1-11.
Tan, Q., Zhan, Y., Gao, S., Fan, W., Chen, J., & Zhong, Y. (2015). Closer the relatives are, more intimate and similar we are: Kinship effects on self-other overlap.Personality and Individual Differences, 73, 7-11.
Xu, F., Bao, X., Fu, G., Talwar, V., & Lee, K. (2010). Lying and truth-telling in children: From concept to action.Child Development, 81(2), 581-596.
Yu, J., Zhu, L., & Leslie, A. M. (2016). Children’s sharing behavior in mini-dictator games: The role of in-group favoritism and theory of mind. Child Development, 87(6), 1747-1757.
Zhang, W., Liang, G., Guo, Z., Liu, Y., & Fan, W. (2022). The more familiar the others, the higher the morality: Children’s preference for familiar others in moral expectations early appears in the negative moral context. Early Child Development and Care, 192(11), 1842-1855.
Zhang, W., Xiang, S., Dai, H., Ren, M., Shen, Y., Fan, W., & Zhong, Y. (2019). The impact of self-relevance on preschool children’s sharing. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 1028.https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01028
Zhu, X., Wang, L., Yang, S., Gu, R., Wu, H., & Luo, Y. (2016). The motivational hierarchy between the personal self and close others in the Chinese brain: An ERP study.Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1467.https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01467
戴雨芊, 邱华, 李红. (2021). 儿童说谎行为的类型与研究范式. 心理学探新, 41(3), 204-213.
费孝通. (1948). 乡土中国. 北京:北京三联书店.
刘璐, 肖雪, 刘丽莎, 徐良苑, 张旭然, 李燕芳. (2019). 儿童在不同卷入情境下基于资源价值的分配行为特点:内群体偏爱的作用. 心理学报, 51(05), 584-597.
陆娟芝, 廖冲, 关青, 罗跃嘉, 崔芳. (2019). 利他与利己动机下的欺骗行为. 心理科学, 42(4), 905-912.
马伟军, 冯睿, 张雅婷, 刘昭远, 王文浩, 汪晨波, 伍佩莹. (2021). “熟人”差序格局及其在记忆中的体现. 心理科学, 44(6), 1446-1452.
谭千保, 邓磊, 张湘一, 常志彬, 欧阳益. (2019). 社会距离与分配差距对儿童有利不公平厌恶的影响. 心理科学, 42(5), 1141-1147.
王益文, 张振, 张蔚, 黄亮, 郭丰波, 原胜. (2014). 群体身份调节最后通牒博弈的公平关注. 心理学报, 46(12),1850-1859.
温芳芳, 佐斌. (2018). 最简群体范式的操作、心理机制及新应用. 心理科学, 41(3), 713-719.
张娜, 刘秀丽. (2015). 3-6岁儿童白谎的概念理解、道德评价与白谎行为的关系. 中国临床心理学杂志, 32(02), 354-357+361.
[1] 郭庆科, 徐萍, 吴睿, 胡姗姗. 群体偏好与年级对小学生利他惩罚行为的影响[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2016, 32(4): 402-408.
Viewed
Full text


Abstract

Cited

  Shared   
  Discussed   
No Suggested Reading articles found!