Psychological Development and Education ›› 2014, Vol. 30 ›› Issue (5): 512-519.
Previous Articles Next Articles
XIE Zhi-long
CLC Number:
Abutalebi, J., Annoni, J. M., Zimine, I., Pegna, A. J., Seghier, M. L., Lee-Jahnke, H., et al. (2008). Language control and lexical competition in bilinguals: an event-related fMRI study. Cerebral Cortex, 18(7), 1496-1505. Abutalebi, J., Della Rosa, P. A., Ding, G., Weekes, B., Costa, A., & Green, D. W. (2012). Language proficiency modulates the engagement of cognitive control areas in multilinguals. Cortex, 49(3), 905-911. Abutalebi, J., & Green, D. (2007). Bilingual language production:The neurocognition of language representation and control. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 20, 242-275. Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Freedman, M. (2007). Bilingualism as a protection against the onset of symptoms of dementia. Neuropsychologia, 45(2), 459-464. Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Green, D. W., & Gollan, T. H. (2009). Bilingual Minds. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 10(3), 89-129. Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M. (2004). Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging, 19(2), 290-303. Bialystok, E., & Martin, M. M. (2004). Attention and inhibition in bilingual children: evidence from the dimensional change card sort task. Developmental Science, 7(3), 325-339. Costa, A., Hernandez, M., Costa-Faidella, J., & Sebastian-Galles, N. (2009). On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: now you see it, now you don't. Cognition, 113(2), 135-149. Green, D. W. (1998). Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1(02), 67-81. Green, D. W. (2011). Language control in different contexts: the behavioral ecology of bilingual speakers. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 103.doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00103 Green, D. W., & Abutalebi, J. (2013). Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 515-530. Marian, V., Blumenfeld, H. K., & Kaushanskaya, M. (2007). The Language Experience and Proficiency Questionnaire(LEAP-Q): Assessing Language Profiles in Bilinguals and Multilinguals. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 940-967. Meuter, R. F. I., & Allport, A. (1999). Bilingual Language Switching in Naming: Asymmetrical Costs of Language Selection. Journal of Memory and Language, 40, 25-40. Mishra, R. K., Hilchey, M. D., Singh, N., & Klein, R. M. (2012). On the time course of exogenous cueing effects in bilinguals: higher proficiency in a second language is associated with more rapid endogenous disengagement. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (Hove), 65(8), 1502-1510. Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex "Frontal Lobe" tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49-100. Paap, K. R., & Greenberg, Z. I. (2013). There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology, 66(2), 232-258. Prior, A., & Gollan, T. H. (2011). Good language-switchers are good task-switchers: evidence from Spanish-English and Mandarin-English bilinguals. Journal of International Neuropsychological Society, 17(4), 682-691. Prior, A., & Macwhinney, B. (2010). A bilingual advantage in task switching. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13(02), 253-262. Rodriguez-Fornells, A., Kramer, U. M., Lorenzo-Seva, U., Festman, J., & Munte, T. F. (2011). Self-assessment of individual differences in language switching. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 388.doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00388 Singh, N., & Mishra, R.K. (2012). Does language proficiency modulate oculomotor control? Evidence from Hindi-English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15(04), 771-781. Singh, N., & Mishra, R. K. (2013). Second language proficiency modulates conflict-monitoring in an oculomotor Stroop task: evidence from Hindi-English bilinguals. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 322.doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00322 Tse, C.-S., & Altarriba, J. (2012). The effects of first-and second-language proficiency on conflict resolution and goal maintenance in bilinguals: Evidence from reaction time distributional analyses in a Stroop task. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15(03), 663-676. Yudes, C., Macizo, P., & Bajo, T. (2011). The influence of expertise in simultaneous interpreting on non-verbal executive processes. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 309.doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00309 李莹丽, 吴思娜, 刘丽虹. (2012). 双语经验对认知能力的影响.心理科学进展, 20(7), 995-1002. |
[1] | LI Jun, WANG Yue, CHEN Xiani, LI Ying. The Effects of Second Language Learning on Cognitive Control and Lexical Access in School-age Children: The Influence of Age and Second Language Proficiency [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2023, 39(2): 219-227. |
[2] | MU Defang, CHEN Yinghe. How Metacognitive Monitoring Influence WCST of Higher Graders in Primary School: The Moderating Role of Executive Function [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2021, 37(3): 305-312. |
[3] | SONG Xiaolei, LI Xiaofang, ZHAO Yuan, HE Dan. The Influence of Cognitive Aging on Conflict Adaptation under Different Task Difficulties [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2017, 33(5): 569-576. |
[4] | MU De-fang, CHEN Ying-he. The Regulative Effect of Cognitive Flexibility on Metacognitive Monitoring and Control [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2013, 29(2): 113-120. |
[5] | DING Yi, JI Ting-ting, CHEN Xu. The Formation Mechanism of Altruistic Punishment and Its Neural Mechanism [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2012, 28(6): 658-664. |
|