Psychological Development and Education ›› 2017, Vol. 33 ›› Issue (5): 569-576.doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2017.05.07

Previous Articles     Next Articles

The Influence of Cognitive Aging on Conflict Adaptation under Different Task Difficulties

SONG Xiaolei, LI Xiaofang, ZHAO Yuan, HE Dan   

  1. School of Psychology, Shaanxi Normal University;Shaanxi Key Laboratory of Behavior and Cognitive Neuroscience, Xi'an 710062
  • Online:2017-09-15 Published:2017-09-15

Abstract: The ability of conflict adaptation reflected not only the ability of conflict monitoring, but also the ability of modulating top-down cognitive control based on conflict situations. The paradigm of task conflictions was adopted in two experiments to explore how the conflict adaptation was influenced by aging problem. Experiment 1 used the classical Flanker task to explore whether the conflict adaptation effect would emerge in both the younger and older adults, and if the conflict adaptation effect was different between them. Experiment 2 integrated the Simon and Flanker conflicts together to examine whether there were some differences between them after increasing the difficulty level. Results indicated that cognitive aging have no influence on conflict adaptation in low difficulty condition, however, when the difficulties of conflict task increased, cognitive resources that older adults recruited from other brain areas can't ever compensate for the damage caused by cognitive aging. Our conclusion is that the influence of cognitive aging on conflict adaptation ability is mainly appeared in high difficulty tasks, which supported compensation theory as well as limited cognitive resources theories.

Key words: flanker conflict, simon conflict, conflict adaptation effect, cognitive aging, cognitive control

CLC Number: 

  • B844

Aisenberg, D., Cohen, N., Pick, H., Tressman, I., Rappaport, M., Shenberg, T., & Henik, A. (2015). Social Priming Improves Cognitive Control in Elderly Adults-Evidence from the Simon Task. Plos One, 10(1), 9-17.

Benikos, N., Johnstone, S. J., & Roodenrys, S. J. (2013). Varying task difficulty in the Go/Nogo task:the effects of inhibitory control, arousal, and perceived effort on ERP components. International Journal of Psychophysiology Official Journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology, 87(3), 262-272.

Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108(3), 624-652.

Cabeza, R. (2002). Hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults:the HAROLD model. Psychology & Aging, 17(1), 85-100.

Castel, A. D., Balota, D. A., Hutchison, K. A., Logan, J. M., & Mj., Y. (2007). Spatial attention and response control in healthy younger and older adults and individuals with Alzheimer's disease:evidence for disproportionate selection impairments in the Simon task. Neuropsychology, 21(2), 170-182.

Chechko, N., Kellermann, T., Schneider, F., & Habel, U. (2014). Conflict adaptation in emotional task underlies the amplification of target. Emotion, 14(2), 321-330.

Clayson, P. E., & Larson, M. J. (2011). Conflict adaptation and sequential trial effects:Support for the conflict monitoring theory. Neuropsychologia, 49(7), 1953-1961.

Cohen, M. X., & Cavanagh, J. F. (2011). Single-Trial Regression Elucidates the Role of Prefrontal Theta Oscillations in Response Conflict. Frontiers in Psychology, 2(4), 30.

Cohen, M. X., & Gaal, S. V. (2013). Dynamic Interactions between Large-Scale Brain Networks Predict Behavioral Adaptation after Perceptual Errors. Cerebral Cortex, 23(5), 1061-1072.

Craik, F. I. M., & Salthouse, T. A. (2010). The handbook of aging and cognition. Applied Cognitive Psychology,15 (5), 585-586.

Egner, T. (2007). Congruency sequence effects and cognitive control. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 7(4), 380-390.

Egner, T. (2008). Multiple conflict-driven control mechanisms in the human brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(10), 374-380.

Egner, T., Delano, M., & Hirsch, J. (2007). Separate conflict-specific cognitive control mechanisms in the human brain. Neuroimage, 35(2), 940-948.

Folstein, J. R., & Van, P. C. (2008). Influence of cognitive control and mismatch on the N2 component of the ERP:a review. Psychophysiology, 45(1), 152-170.

Freitas, A. L., Bahar, M., Yang, S., & Banai, R. (2007). Contextual adjustments in cognitive control across tasks. Psychological Science, 18(12), 1040-1043.

Gratton, G., Coles, M. G. H., & Donchin, E. (1992). Optimizing the use of information:Strategic control of activation of responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology:General, 121(4), 480-506.

Hedden, T., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2004). Insights into the ageing mind:a view from cognitive neuroscience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5(2), 87-96.

Jiang, J., Xiang, L., Zhang, Q., & Chen, A. (2014). Conflict Adaptation Is Independent of Consciousness:Behavioral and ERP Evidence. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 46(5), 581-592.

Jiménez, L., & Méndez, A. (2012). It is not what you expect:dissociating conflict adaptation from expectancies in a Stroop task. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception & Performance, 39(1), 271-284.

Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ:Prentice-Hall.

Kan, I. P., Teubner-Rhodes, S., Drummey, A. B., Nutile, L., Krupa, L., & Novick, J. M. (2013). To adapt or not to adapt:The question of domain-general cognitive control. Cognition, 129(3), 637-651.

Kim, C., Chung, C., & Kim, J. (2010). Multiple cognitive control mechanisms associated with the nature of conflict. Neuroscience Letters, 476(3), 156-160.

Kim, C., Johnson, N. F., & Gold, B. T. (2014). Conflict adaptation in prefrontal cortex:Now you see it, now you don't. Cortex, 50(3), 76-85.

Kray, J., Karbach, J., & Blaye, A. (2012). The influence of stimulus-set size on developmental changes in cognitive control and conflict adaptation. Acta Psychologica, 140(2), 119-128.

Kwee, I. L., & Nakada, T. (2003). Dorsolateral prefrontal lobe activation declines significantly with age——functional NIRS study. Journal of Neurology, 250(5), 525-529.

Larson, M. J., Clayson, P. E., & Baldwin, S. A. (2012). Performance monitoring following conflict:Internal adjustments in cognitive control? Neuropsychologia, 50(3), 426-433.

Larson, M. J., Kaufman, D. A., & Perlstein, W. M. (2009). Conflict adaptation and cognitive control adjustments following traumatic brain injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 15(6), 927-937.

Li, Q., Wang, K., Nan, W., Zheng, Y., Wu, H., Wang, H., & Liu, X. (2015). Electrophysiological dynamics reveal distinct processing of stimulus-stimulus and stimulus-response conflicts. Psychophysiology, 52(4), 562-571.

Li, S. C., & Lindenberger, U. (1999). Cross-level unification:A computational exploration of the link between deterioration of neurotransmitter systems and dedifferentiation of cognitive abilities in old age. Statistics in Medicine, 18(2), 139-154.

Mitchell, D. B., & Perlmutter, M. (1985). Semantic activation and episodic memory:Age similarities and differences. Developmental Psychology, 22(1), 86-94.

Park, D. C., & Reuter-Lorenz, P. (2009). The adaptive brain:Aging and neurocognitive scaffolding. Psychology, 60(60), 173-196.

Proctor, R. W., Pick, D. F., Vu, K. P. L., & Anderson, R. E. (2005). The enhanced Simon effect for older adults is reduced when the irrelevant location information is conveyed by an accessory stimulus. Acta Psychologica, 119(1), 21-40.

Proctor, R. W., Zhang, Y., & Vu, K. P. L. (2013). Older and Younger Adults Show Comparable Affective Compatibility and Simon Effects. Experimental Aging Research, 39(1), 44-69.

Reuterlorenz, P. A., & Cappell, K. A. (2008). Neurocognitive aging and the compensation hypothesis. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17(3), 177-182.

Shaw, R. J. (1992). Age-related increases in the effects of automatic semantic activation. Psychology & Aging, 6(4), 595-604.

Shenhav, A., Botvinick, M. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2013). The expected value of control:an integrative theory of anterior cingulate cortex function. Neuron, 79(2), 217-240.

Tapp, P. D., Siwak, C. T., Gao, F. Q., Chiou, J. Y., Black, S. E., Head, E., Muggenburg, B. A., Cotman, C.W., Milgram, N. W., & Su, M. Y. (2004). Frontal lobe volume, function, and beta-amyloid pathology in a canine model of aging. Journal of Neuroscience the Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 24(38), 8205-8213.

Turner, G. R., & Spreng, R. N. (2012). Executive functions and neurocognitive aging:dissociable patterns of brain activity. Neurobiology of Aging, 33(4), 826.e821-813.

Xue, S., Ren, G., Kong, X., Liu, J., & Qiu, J. (2015). Electrophysiological correlates related to the conflict adaptation effect in an emotional conflict task. Neuroscience Letters, 584, 219-223.

Yeung, N., & Cohen, J. D. (2006). The Impact of Cognitive Deficits on Conflict Monitoring Predictable Dissociations Between the Error-Related Negativity and N2. Psychological Science, 17(2), 164-171.

胡凤培, 王倩, 徐莲, 葛列众, (2012). 基于Flanker、Stroop和Simon多重冲突驱动的认知控制机制. 心理科学, 35(2), 276-281.

蒋军, 向玲, 张庆林, 陈安涛. (2014). 冲突适应独立于意识:来自行为和ERP的证据. 心理学报, 46(5), 581-592.

刘培朵, 杨文静, 田夏, 陈安涛. (2012). 冲突适应效应研究述评. 心理科学进展, 20(4), 532-541.

唐丹丹, 陈安涛. (2013). 冲突适应的神经振荡机制. 中国科学:生命科学, 43(11), 992-1002.

王大华, 黄一帆, 彭华茂, 陈晓敏. (2012). 老年人加工速度的干预研究. 心理学报, 44(4), 469-477.
[1] LI Jun, WANG Yue, CHEN Xiani, LI Ying. The Effects of Second Language Learning on Cognitive Control and Lexical Access in School-age Children: The Influence of Age and Second Language Proficiency [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2023, 39(2): 219-227.
[2] WU Xia, ZHONG Xiping, JIANG Yunpeng. The Effects of Aging on Top-down Attentional Processing under Different Search Situations [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2022, 38(1): 26-34.
[3] MU Defang, CHEN Yinghe. How Metacognitive Monitoring Influence WCST of Higher Graders in Primary School: The Moderating Role of Executive Function [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2021, 37(3): 305-312.
[4] PU Bingyan, PENG Huamao. Older Adults' Decision Process in the Context of Cognitive Aging:Motivation as a Moderator [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2016, 32(1): 114-120.
[5] XIE Zhi-long. Bilingual Language Proficiency and Mental Set Shifting in Cognitive Control [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2014, 30(5): 512-519.
[6] MU De-fang, CHEN Ying-he. The Regulative Effect of Cognitive Flexibility on Metacognitive Monitoring and Control [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2013, 29(2): 113-120.
[7] DING Yi, JI Ting-ting, CHEN Xu. The Formation Mechanism of Altruistic Punishment and Its Neural Mechanism [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2012, 28(6): 658-664.
[8] LI Yong-xin, ZHU Xiang-ru, LI Li. The Conflict Adaptation Effect in 6~12 Years Old Children:Evidence from Flanker Task [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2012, 28(6): 576-580.
[9] GAO Yue, PENG Hua-mao, WEN Jing, WANG Da-hua. The Role of Education in Cognitive Aging [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2011, 27(6): 591-598.
Viewed
Full text


Abstract

Cited

  Shared   
  Discussed   
No Suggested Reading articles found!