Psychological Development and Education ›› 2017, Vol. 33 ›› Issue (3): 273-281.doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2017.03.03

Previous Articles     Next Articles

The Influence Mechanism of Interpersonal Situations on the Joint Simon Effect of Young Children

SONG Xiaolei, LI Yangyang, ZHANG Shiyi, ZHANG Junting   

  1. School of Psychology, Shaanxi Normal University;Shaanxi Key Laboratory of Behavior and Cognitive Neuroscience, Xi'an 710062
  • Online:2017-05-15 Published:2017-05-15

Abstract: Joint Simon effect is used as an effective indicator to examine the co-representation ability for young children. The paradigm of joint Simon task was adopted in the two experiments to explore the development of young children' joint Simon effect and the influence mechanism of interpersonal situations on the effect. The experiment 1, with three age grades from 3 to 5 participated in, was designed to investigate the development of joint Simon effect for young children in order to reveal the development trends of their co-representation ability. Based on the experiment 1, the experiment 2, taking five-year-old children with stable joint Simon effect as the participants, was designed to further examine whether different interpersonal situations would modulate the children's joint Simon effect and what mechanism concealed behind it. The above two experiments indicated that, first, compared to three-year-old children, four-year-old children begin to appear the joint Simon effect but still unstable, and the effect for five-year-old children reaches a more stable level; second, young children's co-representation ability can be influenced by interpersonal situations, and contrasted with the cooperative situation, the ability of their joint action is refrained in the competitive situation. The results of this study, therefore, strongly support the referential coding theory and also broaden the understanding of the co-representation theory.

Key words: interpersonal situation, co-representation, referential coding, young children, joint Simon effect

CLC Number: 

  • B844

Ashley, J., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Cooperative problem solving and teaching in preschoolers. Social Development, 7, 143-163.

Brownell, C. A. (2011). Early developments in joint action. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 2, 193-211.

Brownell, C., Ramani, G.. B., & Zerwas, S. (2006). Becoming a social partner with peers:Cooperation and social understanding in one-and two-year-olds. Child Development, 77, 803-821.

Colzato, L. S., van den Wildenberg W. P. M., & Hommel, B. (2013), Increasing self-other integration through divergent thinking. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20(5), 1011-1016.

Dolk, T., Hommel, B., Colzato, L. S., Schütz-Bosbach, S., Prinz, W., & Liepelt, R. (2014). The joint Simon effect:A review and theoretical integration. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 974.

Dolk, T., Hommel, B., Prinz, W., & Liepelt. (2013). The (not so) social Simon effect:A referential coding account. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 39(5), 1248-1260.

Dolk, T., Liepelt, R., Villringer, A., Prinz, W., & Ragert, P. (2012). Morphometric gray matter differences of the medial frontal cortex influence the social Simon effect. NeuroImage, 61(4), 1249-1254.

Ferraro, L., Iani, C., Mariani, M., Milanese, N., & Rubichi, S. (2011). Facilitation and interference components in the joint Simon task. Experimental Brain Research, 211(3-4), 337-343.

Guagnano, D., Rusconi, E., & Umiltà, C. A. (2010). Sharing a task or sharing space? On the effect of the confederate in action coding in a detection task. Cognition, 114(3), 348-355.

Hamann, K., Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Children's developing commitments to joint goals. Child Development, 83, 137-145.

Hommel, B. (1996). S-R compatibility effects without response uncertainty. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. Section A:Human Experimental Psychology, 49(3), 546-571.

Hommel, B., Colzato, L. S., van den Wildenberg, W. P. M. (2009). How social are task representations? Psychological Science, 20, 794-798.

Humphreys, G. W., & Bedford, J. (2011). The relations between joint action and theory of mind:A neuropsychological analysis. Experimental Brain Research, 211(3-4), 357-369.

Iani, C., Anelli, F., Nicoletti, R., Arcuri, L., & Rubichi, S. (2011). The role of group membership on modulation of joint action. Experimental Brain Research, 211(3-4), 439-445.

Liepelt, R., Wenke, D., & Fisher, R. (2013). Effects of feature integration in a hands-crossed version of the Social Simon paradigm. Psychological Research, 77(2), 240-248.

Liepelt, R., Wenke, D., Fischer, R., & Prinz, W. (2011). Trial-to-trial sequential dependencies in a social and non-social Simon task. Psychological Research, 75(5), 366-375.

Ma, Q., & Shang, Q. (2013). The influence of negative emotion on the Simon effect as reflected by P300. The Scientific World Journal, Article ID 516906.

Meyer, M., Bekkering, H., Paulus, M., & Hunnius, S. (2010). Joint action coordination in 2(1/2)-and 3-year-old children. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4, 220.

Milward, S. J., Kita, S., & Apperly, I. A. (2014). The development of co-representation effects in a joint task:Do children represent a co-actor? Cognition, 132(3), 269-279.

Müller, B. C. N., Brass, M., Kühn, S., Tsai, C.-C., Nieuwboer, W., Dijksterhuis, A., & van Baaren, R., B. (2011a). When Pinocchio acts like a human, a wooden hand becomes embodied. Action co-representation for non-biological agents. Neuropsychologia, 49(5), 1373-1377.

Müller, B. C. N., Kühn, S., van Baaren, R., B., Dotsch, R., Brass, M., & Brass, M. (2011b). Perspective taking eliminates differences in co-representation of out-group members' actions. Experimental Brain Research, 211(3-4), 423-428.

Newman-Norlund, R. D., van Schie, H. T., van Zuijlen, A. M., Bekkering, H. (2007). The mirror neuron system is more active during complementary compared with imitative action. Nature Neuroscience 10:817-818.

Obhi, S. S., & Sebanz, N. (2011). Moving together:Toward understanding the mechanisms of joint action. Experimental Brain Research, 211(3-4), 329-336.

Radke, S., de Lange, F. P., Ullsperger, M., & de Bruijn, E. R. A. (2011). Mistakes that affect others:An fMRI study on processing of own errors in a social context. Experimental Brain Research, 211(3-4), 405-413.

Ruys, K. I., & AAarts, H. (2010). When competition merges people's behavior:Interdependency activates shared action representations. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(6), 1130-1133.

Saby, J. N., Bouquet, C. A., & Marshall, P. J. (2014). Young children co-represent a partner's task:Evidence for a joint Simon effect in five-year-olds. Cognitive Development, 32, 38-45.

Sebanz, N., Bekkering, H., & Knoblich, G. (2006). Joint action:Bodies and minds moving together. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(2), 70-76.

Sebanz, N., Knoblich, G., & Prinz, W. (2003). Representing others' actions:just like one's own? Cognition, 88(3), B11-B21.

Sebanz, N., Knoblich, G., Prinz, W., & Wascher, E. (2006). Twin peaks:An ERP study of action planning and control in coacting individuals. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(5), 859-870.

Sebanz, N., Rebbechi, D., Knoblich, G., Prinz, W., & Frith, C. D. (2007). Is it really my turn? An event-related fMRI study of task sharing. Social Neuroscience, 2(2), 81-95.

Sellaro, R., Dolk, T., Colzato, L. S., Liepelt, R., & Hommel, B. (2015). Referential coding does not rely on location features:Evidence for a nonspatial joint Simon effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Human Perception and Performance, 41(1), 186-195.

Sellaro, R., Treccani, B., Rubichi, S., & Cubelli, R. (2013). When co-action eliminates the Simon effect:disentangling the impact of co-actor's presence and task sharing on joint-task performance. Frontiers in Psychology, 4(844), 1-15.

Stenzel, A., Chinellato, E., Bou, M. A. T., Del Pobil, á. P., Lappe, M., & Liepelt, R. (2012). When humanoid robots become human-like interaction partners:Corepresentation of robotic actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Human Perception and Performance, 38(5), 1073-1077.

Tsai, C. C., Kuo, W. J., Hung, D. L., & Tzeng, O. J. L. (2008). Action corepresentation is tuned to other humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(11), 2015-2024.

Warneken, F., Chen, F., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Cooperative activities in young children and chimpanzees. Child Development, 77, 640-663.

Wen, T., & Hsieh, S. (2015). Neuroimaging of the joint Simon effect with believed biological and non-biological co-actors. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 483.

王力, 张栎文, 张明亮, 陈安涛. (2013). Go/No-Go范式中非反应手状态对Simon效应性质的影响. 心理科学, 36(1), 38-43.

叶浩生. (2012). 镜像神经元:认知具身性的神经生物学证据. 心理学探新, 32(1), 3-7.
[1] LIU Shuang, LI Mei, FENG Xiaohang, ZHANG Xiangkui. A Longitudinal Study of Mastery Motivation and Competence in Early Childhood: The Moderating Effects of Types of Task and Temperament [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2023, 39(2): 161-172.
[2] LIU Tiantian, LI Yan, LI Youjia, JIANG Xinmiao. Co-parenting and Preschool Children's Social Behavior: The Chain Mediating Effects of Parent-child Relationship and Sibling Relationship Quality [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2021, 37(5): 638-647.
[3] LIU Yan, WANG Jing, ZHANG Xiaoyan. The Impact of Self-Projection on Episodic Foresight in Young Children: Evidence from Typically Developing Children and Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2017, 33(6): 649-657.
[4] YANG Lizhu, LI Miao, CHEN Jinghan, SHEN Yue. The Influence of Teacher Expectations on Young Children's Personality: Mediating Effect of Teacher-Children Relationship [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2016, 32(6): 641-648.
[5] LIU Shuang, SUN Yang, ZHANG Xiangkui. The Relationships between Mastery Motivation,Temperament and Competence of Young Children [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2016, 32(3): 265-275.
[6] HUANG Wei, YANG Ze. Goal Orientation, Self-efficacy, and Self-control among 3~6 Years Old Children [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2015, 31(5): 547-554.
[7] XU Fen, GUO Xiao-lin, ZHANG Jia. The Characteristics of 3-4-year-old Children’s Conceptual Knowledge of Lying:the Role of Theory of Mind and Parenting Style [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2013, 29(5): 449-456.
[8] LU Jing, HU Qing-fen. Geometric Cues and its Development and Application in Preschooler’s Ability to Solve Mapping Tasks [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2013, 29(4): 434-440.
[9] JIANG Qin, LI Hong, ZHANG Shu-yue, ZHANG Ting, FENG Ting-yong, LONG Chang-quan. The Role of Perspective-taking in Preschool Children’s Decision-making in Delay of Gratification [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2012, 28(2): 131-139.
[10] DONG Cun-mei. The Cross-Cultural Comparison of Development of Self-Regulation between Japanese and Chinese Young Children [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2012, 28(1): 16-23.
[11] LIU Yan, YANG Li-zhu, DENG Chen-xi. The Development of Foresight and Its Relationship with Inhibitory Control and Theory of Mind [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2012, 28(1): 1-8.
[12] DONG Cun-mei. The Comparison of Development of Self-Regulation between Japanese and Chinese Young Children: The Relationship between Behavioral Responses and Emotional Responses [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2011, 27(2): 133-139.
[13] Lu Xiao-ying, Long Chang-quan, Sun Feng, Xing Dong-liang, Li Hong. The Influence of Induction in 3-year-olds [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2011, 27(1): 25-34.
[14] ZHANG Qi, MA Yan-ping, ZHANG Li. The Occurrence and Development of Young Children’s Informal Reasoning [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2010, 26(6): 592-597,611.
[15] LIU Shuang, ZHANG Xiang-kui. The Effect of Behavior Evaluation on Mastery Motivation in 3-Year-Old Children [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2010, 26(2): 128-132.
Viewed
Full text


Abstract

Cited

  Shared   
  Discussed   
No Suggested Reading articles found!