心理发展与教育 ›› 2012, Vol. 28 ›› Issue (3): 322-328.

• 论文 • 上一篇    下一篇

学前儿童朴素生物学研究新进展:活力因果机制及其应用

苗晶磊, 胡清芬   

  1. 北京师范大学发展心理研究所
  • 出版日期:2012-05-15 发布日期:2012-05-15
  • 通讯作者: 胡清芬,E-mail:cara1235@sina.com E-mail:cara1235@sina.com
  • 基金资助:
    教育部人文社会科学研究青年基金资助项目(10YJCXLX017)

New Advances in Preschool Children’s Naive Biology: Vitalistic Causality and its Application

MIAO Jing-lei, HU Qing-fen   

  1. Institute of Developmental Psychology, Beijing Normal University, Beijing 100875
  • Online:2012-05-15 Published:2012-05-15

摘要: 学前儿童倾向于采用活力因果机制,即用生命力的交换和获取来解释与机体有关的生命现象.活力因果解释介于意图因果解释和机制因果解释之间,被认为是专属于生物领域的一种因果认知机制,它的提出为存在独立的朴素生物学理论提供了强有力的证据.4到6岁是活力机制发展的关键时期.同时,在年长儿童和成人对机体生命现象的认识中,这一机制继续发挥着作用,并不会随着儿童年龄的增长被其它因果机制完全取代.探讨可能导致活力因果机制发展差异性的影响因素以及它与其它因果机制的共同作用将是未来研究的方向.

关键词: 活力因果机制, 生命力, 机体生命现象, 朴素生物学

Abstract: When reasoning about bodily biological phenomenons,preschool children tend to choose vitalistic causality which assumes that series of bodily processes are meant to sustain life by taking in and exchanging vital force.Vitalism is considered to be a biological-specific form of causality which is different from intentional and mechanical causality.It develops fast between 4 and 6 years old and continues to exist in older children' and adults' reasoning.In the future some issues need to be discussed further such as how culture and experience affect the development of vitalism as well as the interactions between vitalism and other types of causality.

Key words: vitalistic causality, vital force, bodily biological phenomenon, naive biology

中图分类号: 

  • B844.1
[1] Backscheider,A.G.,Shatz,M.,& Gelman,S.A.(1993).Preschoolers' ability to distinguish living kinds as a function of regrowth.Child Development,64,1242-1257.
[2] Bloom,P.(1996).Intention,history,and artifact concepts.Cognition,60,1-29.
[3] Carey,S.(1985).Conceptual change in childhood.Cambridge,MA:MIT Press.
[4] Carey,S.(1995).On the origin of causal understanding.In D.Sperber,D.Premack,& A.J.Rremack(Eds.),Causal cognition:A multidisciplinary debate(pp.268-302).New York:Oxford University press.
[5] Gelman,R.(1990).First principles organize attention to and learning about relevant data:Number and the animate-inanimate distinction as examples.Cognitive Science,14,79-106.
[6] Gelman,S.A.(2009).Essentialist reasoning about the biological world.In A.Berthoz,& Y.Christen(Eds.),Neuro-biology of Umwelt:How living beings perceive the world(pp.7-16).Research and Perspectives in Neurosciences.Gelman,S.A.,& Hirschfeld,L.A.(1999).How biological is essentialism? In S.Atran,& D.Medin(Eds.),Folk biology.Cambridge,MA:MIT Press.
[7] Inagaki,K.,& Hatano,G.(1993).Young children's understanding of the mind-body distinction.Child Development,1534-1549.
[8] Inagaki,K.,& Hatano,G.(1994a).Young children's naive theory of biology.Cognition,50,171-188.
[9] Inagaki,K.,& Hatano,G.(1994b).Young children's personifying and vitalistic biology.Cognition,50,1-25.
[10] Inagaki,K.,& Hatano,G.(1996).Young children's recognition of commonalities between animals and plants.Child Development,57,2823-2839.
[11] Inagaki,K.,& Hatano,G.(1999).Children's understanding of mindbody relationships.In M.Siegal,& C.C.Peterson(Eds.),Children's Understanding of Biology and Health(pp.23-44).Cambridge University Press Inagaki,K.,& Hatano,G.(2002).Young children's naive thinking about the biological world.New York:Psychology Press.
[12] Inagaki,K.,& Hatano,G.(2004).Vitalistic causality in young children's naive biology.Trends in Cognitive Science,8,356-362.
[13] Inagaki,K.,& Hatano,G.(2006).Young children's conception of the biological world.Current Directions in Psychological Science,15,177-181.
[14] Jaakkola,R.,& Slaughter,V.(2002).Children's body knowledge:Understanding life as a biological goal.British Journal of Developmental Psychology,20,325-342.
[15] Kelemen,D.(1999).Function,goals and intention:Children's teleological reasoning about objects.Trends in Cognitive Sciences,3,461-468.
[16] Keil,F.C.(1994).The birth and nurturance of concepts by domains:The origins of concepts of living things.In L.Hirschfeld,& S.Gelman(Eds.),Mapping the mind:Domain specificity in cognition and culture.New York:Cambridge University Press.
[17] Keil,F.C.(2003).That's life:Coming to understand biology.Human Development,46,369-377.
[18] Zhu,L.Q.,Liu,G.Y.,& Tardif,T.(2009).Chinese children's explanations for illness.International Journal of Behavioral Development,33,516-519.
[19] Morris,S.C.,Taplin,J.E.,& Gelman,S.A.(2000).Vitalism in naive biological thinking.Developmental Psychology,36,582-595.
[20] Poling,D.A.,& Evans,E.M.(2002).Why do birds of a feather flock together? Developmental change in the use of multiple explanations:intention,teleology and essentialism.British Journal of Developmental Psychology,20,112.
[21] Spelke,E.S.,Phillips,A.,& Woodward,A.L.(1995).Infants' knowledge of object motion and human action.In D.Sperber,D.Premack,& A.Premack(Eds.),Causal cognition:A multidisciplinary debate(pp.44-78).Oxford:Claredon Press.
[22] Slaughter,V.(2005).Young children's understanding of death.Australian Psychologist,40,179-186.
[23] Slaughter,V.,Jaakkola,R.,& Carey,S.(1999).Constructing a coherent theory:Children's biological understanding of life and death.
[24] In M.Siegal,& C.Petersen(Eds.),Children's Understanding of biology and health(pp.71-76).Cambridge:Cambridge University Press.
[25] Slaughter,V.,& Lyons,M.(2003).Learning about life and death in early childhood.Cognitive Psychology,46,1-30.
[26] Slaughter,V.,& Ting,C.(2010).Development of ideas about food and nutrition from preschool to university.Appetite,55,556-564.
[27] Toyama,N.(2010).Japanese children's and adults' awareness of psychogenic bodily reactions.International Journal of Behavioral Development,34,1-9.
[28] Wellman,H.M.,& Gelman,S.A.(1992).Cognitive development:Foundational theories and core domains.Annual Review of Psychology,43,337-375.
[29] Waxman,S.,Medin,D.,& Ross,N.(2007).Folk biological reasoning from a cross-cultural developmental perspective:Early essentialist notions are shaped by cultural beliefs.Developmental Psychology,43,294-308
[30] 方富熹.(1985).3-5岁儿童关于认知生物和非生物的实验研究.心理学报,17,62-70.
[31] 朱莉琪,方富熹.(2000).学前儿童"朴素生物学理论”发展的实验研究.心理学报,32,177-182.
[32] 朱莉琪,方富熹.(1999).学前儿童朴素生物学理论的研究.心理学动态,7,31-36.
[1] 周海丽, 周晖. 4~7岁儿童基于不同可信度特质和情景的信任判断[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2014, 30(6): 570-576.
[2] 朱颖, 刘亚鹏, 张光珍, 张印平, 梁宗保, 叶明, 邓慧华. 儿童早期情绪性对学步期父母养育方式的影响[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2014, 30(6): 577-584.
[3] 李燕芳, 徐良苑, 吕莹, 刘丽君, 王耘. 母子关系、师幼关系与学前流动儿童的社会适应行为[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2014, 30(6): 624-634.
[4] 卢淳, 郭红力, 司继伟, 孙燕. 不同数字线下儿童与成人分数估计的表征模式[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2014, 30(5): 449-456.
[5] 刘妮娜, 仝文, 闫国利. 4~7岁儿童汉字正字法意识的发展[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2014, 30(5): 457-465.
[6] 陈光辉. 儿童欺负/受欺负与同伴网络的关系:p*模型分析[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2014, 30(5): 474-481.
[7] 陆凤英, 汤永隆, 曹梦露, 刘晓军. 儿童虐待与戒毒动机:自我概念的中介效应[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2014, 30(5): 527-532.
[8] 马凤玲, 汤玉龙, 郑婷婷, 徐芬. 3~5岁幼儿基于面孔的信任判断的发展[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2014, 30(4): 337-344.
[9] 吴海珍, 赵蕾, 卢英俊. 莫扎特音乐对幼儿时空推理能力影响的研究[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2014, 30(4): 345-354.
[10] 钟佑洁, 李艳华, 张进辅. 儿童攻击行为的短期追踪干预研究:从社会信息加工的角度[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2014, 30(3): 225-233.
[11] 李占星, 曹贤才, 庞维国, 牛玉柏. 6~10岁儿童对损人情境下行为者的道德情绪判断与归因[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2014, 30(3): 252-258.
[12] 古丽努尔·阿扎提, 苏彦捷, 于文. 孤独症个体的谎言理解及其与对自我/他人错误信念理解的关系[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2014, 30(2): 113-120.
[13] 聂晋文, 芦咏莉. 父亲参与对儿童延迟满足能力的影响:儿童性别的调节作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2014, 30(2): 121-128.
[14] 郝振, 崔丽娟. 受歧视知觉对流动儿童社会融入的影响:中介机制及自尊的调节作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2014, 30(2): 137-144.
[15] 倪士光, 李虹. 流动儿童认同整合与歧视知觉的关系:社会支持和应对方式的作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2014, 30(1): 31-38.
Viewed
Full text


Abstract

Cited

  Shared   
  Discussed   
No Suggested Reading articles found!