心理发展与教育 ›› 2022, Vol. 38 ›› Issue (4): 457-465.doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2022.04.01
• 认知与社会性发展 • 下一篇
张锋1, 尹华站2, 靳凯歌1, 臧鑫磊1, 朱芷毅1, 赵俊峰1
ZHANG Feng1, YIN Huazhan2, JIN Kaige1, ZANG Xinlei1, ZHU Zhiyi1, ZHAO Junfeng1
摘要: 采用时距二分任务,探讨聋生在视觉通道的1s以下和1s以上时距知觉的特点。结果发现,在1s以下条件下,聋生时距判断准确性低于普通学生;聋生CNV峰值与LPCt波幅低于普通学生,CNV潜伏期短于普通学生,LPCt峰值潜伏期长于普通学生。在1s以上条件下,聋生时距判断准确性高于普通学生;聋生N1、P2、CNV与LPCt成分各项指标与普通学生均没有显著差异。这说明,听觉丧失损伤了聋生1s以下的时距记忆与决策过程,支持了普遍缺陷假设;但听觉丧失未对聋生1s以上时距加工产生显著影响,支持了感觉补偿机制。因此,时距长度在听觉丧失对视觉时距知觉的影响中具有调节作用,为时间认知的分段综合模型提供了新的支持证据。
中图分类号:
Bannier, D.,Wearden, J., Le Dantec, C. C., & Rebai, M. (2019). Differences in the temporal processing between identification and categorization of durations:A behavioral and ERP study. Behavioural Brain Research, 356, 197-203. Bolognini, N., Cecchetto, C., Geraci, C., Maravita, A., Pascual-Leone, A., & Papagno, C. (2012). Hearing shapes our perception of time:Temporal discrimination of tactile stimuli in deaf people.Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24(2), 276-286. Bottari, D., Caclin, A., Giard, M.H., & Pavani, F. (2011). Changes in early cortical visualprocessing predict enhanced reactivity in deaf individuals. PLoS ONE, 6(9), e25607.https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0025607 Bottari, D., Nava, E., Ley, P., & Pavani, F. (2010). Enhanced reactivity to visual stimuli in deaf individuals.Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, 28(2), 167-179. Fraisse, P. (1984). Perception and estimation of time.Annual Review of Psychology, 35(1), 1-37. Gibbon, J., Church, R. M., & Meck, W. H. (1984). Scalar timing in memory. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 423(1), 52-77. Hillyard, S. A., Hink, R. F., Schwent, V. L., & Picton, T. W. (1973). Electrical signs of selective attention in the human brain.Science, 182(4108), 177-180. Iversen, J. R., Patel, A. D., Nicodemus, B., & Emmorey, K. (2015). Synchronization to auditory and visual rhythms in hearing and deaf individuals. Cognition, 134, 232-244. Kowalska, J., & Szelag, E. (2006). The effect of congenital deafness on duration judgment.Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 47(9), 946-953. Lewis, P. A., & Miall, R. C. (2003a). Brain activation patterns during measurement of sub- and supra-second intervals.Neuropsychologia, 41(12), 1583-1592. Lewis, P. A., & Miall, R. C. (2003b). Distinct systems for automatic and cognitively controlled time measurement:Evidence from neuroimaging.Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 13(2), 250-255. Lijffijt, M., Lane, S. D., Meier, S. L., Boutros, N. N., Burroughs, S., Steinberg, J. L., … Swann, A. C. (2009). P50, N100, and P200 sensory gating:Relationships with behavioral inhibition, attention, and working memory. Psychophysiology, 46(5), 1059-1068. Lindbergh, C. A., & Kieffaber, P. D. (2012). The neural correlates of temporal judgments in the duration bisection task.Neuropsychologia, 51(2), 191-196. Macar, F., & Vidal, F. (2003). The CNV peak:An index of decision making and temporal memory.Psychophysiology, 40(6), 950-954. Morein-Zamir, S., Soto-Faraco, S., & Kingstone, A. (2003). Auditory capture of vision:Examining temporal ventriloquism.Cognitive Brain Research, 17(1), 154-163. Myklebust, H. R. (1964).The Psychology of Deafness. Grune and Stratton. Nava, E., Bottari, D., Zampini, M., & Pavani, F. (2008). Visual temporal order judgment in profoundly deaf individuals.Experimental Brain Research, 190(2), 179-188. Ng, K. K., Tobin, S., & Penney, T. B. (2011). Temporal accumulation and decision processes in the duration bisection task revealed by contingent negative variation.Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 5, 77.https://doi.org/10.3389/fnint.2011.00077 Paul, I., Wearden, J., Bannier, D., Gontier, E., Le Dantec, C., & Rebaï, M. (2011). Making decisions about time:Event-related potentials and judgments about the equality of durations. Biological Psychology, 88(1), 94-103. Pfeuty, M., Ragot, R., & Pouthas, V. (2003).When time is up:CNV time course differentiates the roles of the hemispheres in the discrimination of short tone durations. Experimental Brain Research, 151(3), 372-379. Pfeuty, M., Ragot, R., & Pouthas, V. (2005). Relationship between CNV and timing of an upcoming event. Neuroscience Letters, 382(1-2), 106-111. Pouthas, V., Garnero, L., Ferrandez, A. M., & Renault, B. (2000). ERPs and PET analysis of time perception:Spatial and temporal brain mapping during visual discrimination tasks. Human Brain Mapping, 10(2), 49-60. San Martin, R., Appelbaum, L. G., Pearson, J. M., Huettel, S. A., & Woldorff, M. G. (2013). Rapid brain responses independently predict gain maximization and loss minimization during economic decision making. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(16), 7011-7019. Tharpe, A., Ashmead, D., & Rothpletz, A. (2002). Visual attention in children with normal hearing, children with hearing aids, and children with cochlear implants. Journal of Speech, Hearing and Language Research, 45, 403-413. Tirinelli, G., Brunetti, R., & Olivetti-Belardinelli, M. (2009).Time reproduction of structured auditory events by deaf and hearing subjects. In J. Louhivuori, T. Eerola, S. Saarikallio, T. Himberg, & P-S. Eerola (Eds.). Proceedings of the 7th triennial conference of European society for the cognitive sciences of music (pp. 518-525), Jyväskylä. Wearden, J. H. (1991). Human performance on an analogue of an interval bisection task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section B, 43(1), 59-81. Wearden, J., H., & Ferrara, A. (1995). Stimulus spacing effects in temporal bisection by humans.The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section B, 48(4), 289-310. Wiener, M., & Thompson, J. C. (2015). Repetition enhancement and memory effects for duration.NeuroImage, 113, 268-278. Yuan, J., Xu, S., Yang, J., Liu, Q., Chen, A., Zhu, L., … Li, H. (2011). Pleasant mood intensifies brain processing of cognitive control:ERP correlates.Biological Psychology, 87(1), 17-24. Zhang, F., Jin, K., & Zhang, S. (2020). Visual duration bisection in profoundly deaf individuals.PeerJ, 8, e10133.https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.10133 甘甜, 罗跃嘉, 张志杰. (2009). 情绪对时间知觉的影响. 心理科学, 32(4), 836-839. 胡湘明, 黄希庭. (1992). 短时距知觉的视听通道效应的初步研究. 心理科学, 15(1), 6-10. 黄顺航, 刘培朵, 李庆庆, 陈有国, 黄希庭. (2018). 疼痛表情对秒下及秒上时距知觉的影响. 心理科学, 41(2), 278-284. 黄希庭, 李伯约, 张志杰. (2003). 时间认知分段综合模型的探讨. 西南师范大学学报(人文社会科学版), 29(2), 5-9. 黄希庭, 郑云. (1993). 时间判断的视听通道效应的实验研究. 心理学报, 25(3), 3-10. 陶丹, 李朋, 宣宾, 周晶, 李明珠. (2018). 临床抑郁症和阈下抑郁个体的时间知觉模式及其神经机制. 科学通报, 63(20), 2036-2047. 王庭照. (2007). 聋人与听力正常人图形视知觉加工能力的比较实验研究——基于拓扑性质知觉理论的探讨(博士学位论文). 华东师范大学, 上海. 王振勇, 李宏翰, 黄希庭. (1999). 时间信息视听通道效应的实验检验. 心理学动态, 17(3), 76-80. 尹华站, 李丹, 陈盈羽, 黄希庭. (2016). 1~6 秒时距认知分段性特征. 心理学报, 48(9),1119-1129. 尹华站, 李丹, 陈盈羽, 黄希庭. (2017). 1s范围视听时距认知的分段性研究. 心理科学, 40(2),321-328. 尹华站, 李祚山, 李丹, 黄希庭. (2010). 时距加工"长度效应"研究述评. 心理科学进展, 18(6), 887-891. 张锋, 赵国祥. (2019). 情绪对聋哑大学生时距知觉的影响. 心理科学, 42(4), 861-867. 张凤琴, 王庭照, 方俊明. (2005). 听觉经验缺失对时距估计影响的实验研究. 心理科学, 28(4), 806-808. 张志杰, 尹华站, 黄希庭. (2007). 时间知觉和关联负变化. 心理科学进展, 15(2), 308-312. |
[1] | 陈朝阳, 刘志方, 苏永强, 程亚华. 高低阅读技能聋生词汇加工的语境预测性效应特点:眼动证据[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2018, 34(6): 692-699. |
[2] | 陶云, 马谐. 面孔情绪下3~8岁儿童时距知觉的实验研究[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2010, 26(3): 225-232. |
[3] | 陶云, 马谐. 面孔情绪下3~8岁儿童时距知觉的实验研究[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2010, 26(3): 225-232. |
[4] | 陶云, 马谐. 面孔情绪下3~8岁儿童时距知觉的实验研究[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2010, 26(3): 225-232. |
|