Psychological Development and Education ›› 2022, Vol. 38 ›› Issue (1): 26-34.doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2022.01.04

Previous Articles     Next Articles

The Effects of Aging on Top-down Attentional Processing under Different Search Situations

WU Xia1,2,3, ZHONG Xiping2, JIANG Yunpeng1,2,3   

  1. 1. Key Research Base of Humanities and Social Sciences of the Ministry of Education, Academy of Psychology and Behavior, Tianjin Normal University, Tianjin 300387;
    2. Faculty of Psychology, Tianjin Normal University, Tianjin 300387;
    3. Tianjin Social Science Laboratory of Students'Mental Development and Learning, Tianjin 300387
  • Published:2022-02-17

Abstract: In visual search, top-down and bottom-up processing can flexibly capture task-relative information and inhibit task-irrelative information according to different search situations. Specifically, the top-down processing can be enhanced when target is a salient stimulus among distractors, while the bottom-up processing can be enhanced when the target is not the salient stimulus. However, it is unclear that whether older people can adopt different attentional processing under different target situations as young people. In order to investigate the effects of aging on the flexibility of attention, the present study employed young and old people to participate a spatial cue target paradigm. Participants were instructed to search for a predefined color target and the target displays were preceded by spatially unpredicted color singleton cues. Color consistency of the distractors (consistent vs. inconsistent) was manipulated to change the salience situations of the target, Matching level of cue and target (task-relative vs. task-irrelative) was manipulated to investigate the top-down and bottom-up processing, Validity of cue (valid vs. invalid) was manipulated to measure the effects of attentional capture and inhibition. Results showed that, for young people, the effects of attentional capture for task-relative cues in salient situation was significantly smaller than that in the non-salient situation, suggesting that the young people can flexibly regulate the attentional processing according to different target situations. However, the result was disappeared for elders, indicating a lack of flexibility with aging. More importantly, the results of the IES (RT/ACC) showed a cue validity effect for the task-relative cue and a reversed cue validity effect for the task-irrelative cue in elder, indicating the remaining effects of attentional capture and inhibition of aging. In sum, the present study reveals that aging can decrease the flexibility of cognitive control, but still can retain the attentional capture for task-relative stimulus and the attentional inhibition for task-irrelative stimulus, which challenges the impaired inhibition ability of aging.

Key words: cognitive aging, top-down processing, attentional capture, attentional inhibition, spatial cue-target paradigm

CLC Number: 

  • B844
Amenedo, E., Lorenzo-lópez, L., & Pazo-álvarez, P. (2012). Response processing during visual search in normal aging:The need for more time to prevent cross talk between spatial attention and manual response selection. Biological Psychology, 91(2), 201-211.
Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Snyder, A. Z., Vincent, J. L., Lustig, C., Head, D.,Raichle, M. E., & Buckner, R. L. (2007). Disruption of large-scale brain systems in advanced aging. Neuron, 56, 924-935.
Ansorge, U.,Priess, H., & Kerzel, D. (2013). Effects of relevant and irrelevant color singletons on inhibition of return and attentional capture. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 75(8), 1687-1702.
Ashinoff, B. K., Geng, J. J., & Mevorach, C. (2019). Delayed Reactive Distractor Suppression in Aging Populations. Psychology and Aging, 34(3), 481-430.
Bacon, W. F., & Egeth, H. E. (1994). Overriding stimulus-driven attentional capture. Perception & Psychophysics, 55(5), 485-496.
Bélanger, S., Belleville, S., & Gauthier, S. (2010). Inhibition impairments in Alzheimer's disease, mild cognitive impairment and healthy aging:Effect of congruency proportion in a Stroop task. Neuropsychologia, 48(2), 581-590.
Bennett, I. J.,Golob, E. J., & Starr, A. (2004). Age-related differences in auditory event-related potentials during a cued attention task. Clinical Neurophysiology, 115(13), 2602-2615.
Braver, T. S. (2012). The variable nature of cognitive control:A dual mechanisms framework.Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(2), 106-113.
Bruyer, R., & Brysbaert, M. (2011). Combining speed and accuracy in cognitive psychology:Is the inverse efficiency score (IES) a better dependent variable than the mean reaction time (RT) and the percentage of errors (PE)? Psychologica Belgica, 51(1), 5-13.
Campbell, K. L., Grady, C. L., Ng, C., & Hasher, L. (2012). Age differences in the frontoparietal cognitive control network:Implications for distractibility.Neuropsychologia, 50(9), 2212-2223.
Cassavaugh, N. D., Kramer, A. F., & Irwin, D. E. (2003). Influence of task-irrelevant onset distractors on the visual search performance of young and old adults. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 10(1), 44-60.
Chou, Y., Chen, N., & Madden, D. J. (2013). Functional brain connectivity and cognition:Effects of adult age and task demands.Neurobiology of Aging, 34, 1925-1934.
Cid-Fernández, S., LindÃ-n, M., & DÃ-az, F. (2014). Effects of aging and involuntary capture of attention on event-related potentials associated with the processing of and the response to a target stimulus. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8,745.https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00745
Davis, S. W., Dennis, N. A.,Daselaar, S. M., Fleck, M. S., & Cabeza, R. (2008). Que PASA? The posterior-anterior shift in aging. Cerebral Cortex, 18(5), 1201-1209.
Deslauriers, J., Ansado, J., Marrelec, G., Provost, J. S., & Joanette, Y. (2017). Increase of posterior connectivity in aging within the Ventral Attention Network:A functional connectivity analysis using independent component analysis. Brain Research, 1657, 288-296.
Eimer, M., & Kiss, M. (2010). Top-down search strategies determine attentional capture in visual search:Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 72(4), 951-962.
Eimer, M., Kiss, M., & Cheung, T. (2010). Priming of pop-out modulates attentional target selection in visual search:Behavioural and electrophysiological evidence. Vision Research, 50(14), 1353-1361.
Folk, C. L., & Hoyer, W. J. (1992). Aging and shifts of visual spatial attention. Psychology and Aging, 7(3), 453-465.
Folk, C. L., & Remington, R. (1998). Selectivity in Distraction by Irrelevant Featural Singletons:Evidence for Two Forms of Attentional Capture.Journal of Experimental Psychology:Human Perception and Performance, 24(3), 847-858.
Folk, C. L., & Remington, R. W. (2015). Unexpected abrupt onsets can override a top-down set for color.Journal of Experimental Psychology:Human Perception and Performance, 41(4), 1153-1165.
Gazzaley, A., Clapp, W., Kelley, J.,Mcevoy, K., Knight, R. T., & Esposito, M. D. (2008). Age-related top-down suppression deficit in the early stages of cortical visual memory processing. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 105(35), 13122-13126.
Hickey, C.,Lollo, V. Di, & Mcdonald, J. J. (2008). Electrophysiological indices of target and distractor processing in visual search. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(4), 760-775.
Jernigan, T. L., Archibald, S. L.,Fennema-Notestine, C., Gamst, A. C., Stout, J. C., Bonner, J., & Hesselink, J. R. (2001). Effects of age on tissues and regions of the cerebrum and cerebellum. Neurobiology of Aging, 22(4), 581-594.
Kawahara, J. (2010). Identifying a "default" visual search mode with operant conditioning. Acta Psychologica, 135(1), 38-49.
Kiss, M.,Grubert, A., Petersen, A., & Eimer, M. (2012). Attentional capture by salient distractors during visual search is determined by temporal task demands. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24(3), 749-759.
Kleerekooper, I., van Rooij, S. J. H., van Den Wildenberg, W. P. M., de Leeuw, M., Kahn, R. S., & Vink, M. (2016). The effect of aging on fronto-striatal reactive and proactive inhibitory control. NeuroImage, 132(2), 51-58.
Kropotov, J., Ponomarev, V., Tereshchenko, E. P., Müller, A., & Jäncke, L. (2016). Effect of aging on ERP components of cognitive control. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 8, 69.https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2016.00069
Lamy, D., Carmel, T.,Egeth, H. E., & Leber, A. B. (2006). Effects of search mode and intertrial priming on singleton search. Perception and Psychophysics, 68(6), 919-932.
Lamy, D., & Egeth, H. E. (2003). Attentional capture in singleton-detection and feature-search modes. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Human Perception and Performance, 29(5), 1003-1020.
Li, L., Gratton, C.,Fabiani, M., & Knight, R. T. (2013). Age-related frontoparietal changes during the control of bottom-up and top-down attention:An ERP study. Neurobiology of Aging, 34(2), 477-488.
Lien, M.,Gemperle, A., & Ruthruff, E. (2011). Aging and involuntary attention capture:Electrophysiological evidence for preserved attentional control with advanced age. Psychology and Aging, 26(1), 188-203.
Li, S. C., & Lindenberger, U. (1999). Cross-level unification:a computational exploration of the link between deterioration of neurotransmitter systems and dedifferentiation of cognitive abilities in old age. In L. G. Nilsson, & H. Markowitsch (Eds.), Cognitive neuroscience of memory (pp.103-146). Hogrefe and Huber.
Madden, D. J., Parks, E. L., Davis, S. W., Diaz, M. T., Potter, G. G., Chou, Y. H., … Cabeza, R. (2014). Age mediation of frontoparietal activation during visual feature search.NeuroImage, 102(P2), 262-274.
Manard, M., Franc, S., Phillips, C., & Salmon, E. (2017). The neural bases of proactive and reactive control processes in normal aging. Behavioural Brain Research, 320, 504-516.
Mattson, M. P., Duan, W., Chan, S. L., Cheng, A.,Haughey, N., Gary, D. S., … Furukawa, K. (2002). Neuroprotective and neurorestorative signal transduction mechanisms in brain aging:Modification by genes, diet and behavior. Neurobiology of Aging, 23(5), 695-705.
Mertes, C., Wascher, E., & Schneider, D. (2017). Compliance instead of flexibility? On age-related differences in cognitive control during visual search. Neurobiology of Aging, 53, 169-180.
Monge, Z. A.,Geib, B. R., Siciliano, R. E., Packard, L. E., Tallman, W., & Madden, D. J. (2017). Functional modular architecture underlying attentional control in aging. NeuroImage, 155, 257-270.
Nasreddine, Z. S., Phillips, N. A., Charbonneau, S., Whitehead, V., Collin, I., Cummings, J. L., & Chertkow, H. (2005). The montreal cognitive assessment, MoCA:A brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 53(4), 695-699.
O'Sullivan, M., Jones, D. K., Summers, P. E., Morris, R. G., Williams, S. C. R., & Markus, H. S. (2001). Evidence for cortical "disconnection" as a mechanism of age-related cognitive decline.Neurology, 57(4), 632-638.
Phillips, S., & Takeda, Y. (2010). Frontal-parietal synchrony in elderly EEG for visual search.International Journal of Psychophysiology, 75(1), 39-43.
Reuter-lorenz, P. A., & Cappell, K. A. (2008). Neurocognitive aging and the compensation hypothesis. Association for Psychological Science, 17(3), 177-182.
Salthouse, T. A. (1996). The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition.Psychlolgical Review, 103(3), 403-428.
Spreng, R. N., Stevens, W. D., Viviano, J. D., & Schacter, D. L. (2016). Attenuated anticorrelation between the default and dorsal attention networks with aging:Evidence from task and rest. Neurobiology of Aging, 45, 149-160.
Theeuwes, J. (1991). Exogenous and endogenous control of attention:The effect of visual onsets and offsets. Perception & Psychophysics, 49(1), 83-90.
Theeuwes, J. (1992). Perceptual selectivity for color and form. Perception & Psychophysics, 51(6), 599-606.
Theeuwes, J. (2004). Top-down search strategies cannot override attentional capture. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11(1), 65-70.
Theeuwes, J. (2010). Top-down and bottom-up control of visual selection. Acta Psychologica, 135(2), 77-99.
Tsvetanov, K. A.,Mevorach, C., Allen, H., & Humphreys, G. W. (2013). Age-related differences in selection by visual saliency. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 75(7), 1382-1394.
Veen, V. V., & Carter, C. S. (2002). The anterior cingulate as a conflict monitor:fMRI and ERP studies. Physiology & Behavior, 77(4-5), 477-482.
Wiegand, I., Finke, K., Müller, H. J., & Töllner, T. (2013). Event-related potentials dissociate perceptual from response-related age effects in visual search. Neurobiology of Aging, 34(3), 973-985.
刘丽, 白学军. (2016). 注意控制定势和线索类型在注意捕获中的作用. 心理学报, 48(9), 1093-1104.
王炜, 刘丹丹, 高中宝, 解恒革, 周波, 陈彤, 张晓红. (2010). 蒙特利尔认知评估量表(中文版)在驻京军队离退休干部中界值划分的初步研究. 中华保健医学杂志, 12(4), 271-273.
[1] PAN Yi, ZHANG Lin. Attentional Capture by Entirely Irrelevant Stimuli Driven by the Contents of Working Memory [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2019, 35(5): 522-529.
[2] SONG Xiaolei, LI Xiaofang, ZHAO Yuan, HE Dan. The Influence of Cognitive Aging on Conflict Adaptation under Different Task Difficulties [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2017, 33(5): 569-576.
[3] PU Bingyan, PENG Huamao. Older Adults' Decision Process in the Context of Cognitive Aging:Motivation as a Moderator [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2016, 32(1): 114-120.
[4] GAO Yue, PENG Hua-mao, WEN Jing, WANG Da-hua. The Role of Education in Cognitive Aging [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2011, 27(6): 591-598.
Viewed
Full text


Abstract

Cited

  Shared   
  Discussed   
No Suggested Reading articles found!