Psychological Development and Education ›› 2020, Vol. 36 ›› Issue (2): 138-145.doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2020.02.02

Previous Articles     Next Articles

The Underlying Processing Mechanism of the Aftereffects on Prospective Memory: Evidence from Eye Movements

XIN Cong1, ZHANG Manman1, GUO Yingxiu1, GUO Yunfei2, CHEN Youzhen1   

  1. 1. School of Psychology, Fujian Normal University, Fuzhou 350117;
    2. Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing 400715
  • Published:2020-03-19

Abstract: Prospective memory (PM) is the ability of remembering to perform an intended action in the future. The aftereffects of prospective memory is defined as the phenomenon that the completed intention has an influence on the performance of the ongoing task. Based on the repeated prospective memory target paradigm and visual search paradigm, the current study embedded a prospective memory task in a visual search task in which multi-targets would be presented in the display. A 2(condition)×5(trial type) mixed factorial design was adopted. In the salient condition, the PM target appeared in red font against a black background screen. In the non-salient condition, PM targets were presented in a white font against background. The results show that the reaction time of original PM target in the salient and non-salient conditions was slower than the control condition during the finished-PM phase, and the PM target being in front of the OT target was slower than the PM target being behind the OT target. In addition, the percentage of commission errors was low in all conditions. The eye movement data showed that there was no significant difference between the PM target being in front of the OT target and the PM target being behind the OT target in the non-salient condition; The quantity of fixations in the salient condition were more than the control condition, and the quantity of fixations in the PM target being in front of OT target were more than the PM target being behind the OT target. In addition, both the first and total fixation durations were no significant difference across different conditions and trail types. In sum, the results suggest participants would allocate cognitive resources to suppress the original PM target during the finished-PM phase, supporting the inhibition processing.

Key words: the aftereffects of prospective memory, spontaneous retrieval processing, strategic monitoring processing, inhibition processing, eye tracking

CLC Number: 

  • B844
Anderson, F. T., & Einstein, G. O. (2016). The fate of completed intentions. Memory, 25(4),467-480.
Boywitt, C. D., Rummel, J., & Meiser, T. (2015). Commission errors of active intentions:The roles of aging, cognitive load, and practice. Aging Neuropsychology and Cognition, 22(5),560-576.
Bugg, J. M., & Scullin, M. K. (2013). Controlling intentions:The surprising ease of stopping after going relative to stopping after never having gone. Psychological science, 24(12), 2463-2471.
Bugg, J. M., Scullin, M. K., & McDaniel, M. A. (2013). Strengthening encoding via implementation intention formation increases prospective memory commission errors. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 20(3),522-527.
Bugg, J. M., Scullin, M. K., & Rauvola, R. S. (2016). Forgetting no-longer-relevant prospective memory intentions is (sometimes) harder with age but easier with forgetting practice. Psychology and Aging, 31(4),358-369.
Dasse, M. N. (2016). Investigating prospective memory commission errors(Unpublished doctorial dissertation). Baylor University, Texas.
Einstein, G. O., & McDaniel, M. A. (1990). Normal aging and prospective memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition, 16(4), 717-726.
Kimmel, S. E., Chen, Z., Price, M., Parker, C. S., Metlay, J. P., & Christie, J. D. (2007). The influence of patient adherence on anticoagulation control with warfarin:Results from the international normalized ratio adherence and genetics (in-range) study. Archives of Internal Medicine, 167(3), 229-235.
Meiser, T. & Rummel, J. (2012). False prospective memory responses as indications of automatic processes in the initiation of delayed intentions. Consciousness and Cognition, 21(3), 1509-1516.
Pink, J. E., & Dodson, C. S. (2013). Negative prospective memory:Remembering not to perform an action. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 20(1), 184-190.
Rose, F. D., Attree, E. A., Brooks, B. M., Parslow, D. M., Penn, P. R., & Ambihaipahan, N. (2000). Training in virtual environments:Transfer to real world tasks and equivalence to real task training. Ergonomics, 43(4), 494-511.
Schaper, P., & Grundgeiger, T. (2016). Commission errors in delay-execute prospective memory tasks. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70(8), 1423-1438.
Scott, I. A. (2016). The role of context and memory strategies in prospective memory commission errors(Unpublished doctorial dissertation). Tennessee University, Chattanooga.
Scullin, M. K., & Bugg, J. M. (2013). Failing to forget:Prospective memory commission errors can result from spontaneous retrieval and impaired executive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition, 39(3), 965-977.
Scullin, M. K., Bugg, J. M., & McDaniel, M. A. (2012). Whoops, I did it again:Commission errors in prospective memory. Psychology and Aging, 27(1),46-53.
Scullin, M. K., Bugg, J. M., McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. (2011). Prospective memory and aging:Preserved spontaneous retrieval, but impaired deactivation, in older adults. Memory and Cognition, 39(7),1232-1240.
Scullin, M. K., Einstein, G. O., & McDaniel, M. A. (2009). Evidence for spontaneous retrieval of suspended but not finished prospective memories. Memory and Cognition, 37(4), 425-433.
Van Benthem, K. D., Herdman, C. M., Tolton, R. G., & Lefevre, J. A. (2015). Prospective memory failures in aviation:Effects of cue salience, workload, and individual differences. Aerospace Medicine and Human Performance, 84(4), 366-373.
Walser, M., Fischer, R., & Goschke, T. (2012). The failure of deactivating intentions:Aftereffects of completed intentions in the repeated prospective memory cue paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38(4),1030-1044.
Walser, M., Goschke, T., & Fischer, R. (2014). The difficulty of letting go:Moderators of the deactivation of completed intentions. Psychological Research, 78(4), 574-583.
Walser, M., Goschke, T., Möschl, M., & Fischer, R. (2016). Intention deactivation:Effects of prospective memory task similarity on aftereffects of completed intentions. Psychological Research, 81(5), 961-981.
West, R., Mcnerney, M. W., & Travers, S. (2007). Gone but not forgotten:The effects of cancelled intentions on the neural correlates of prospective memory. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 64(3), 215-225.
Willis, M. (2016). Intention Interference, Intention Superiority and Commission Errors in Prospective Memory Tasks after Suspended or Finished Instructions(Unpublished doctorial dissertation). Baylor University, Texas.
陈丽君, 郑雪. (2014). 大学生问题发现过程的眼动研究. 心理学报, 46(3),367-384.
陈思佚, 周仁来. (2010). 前瞻记忆需要经过策略加工:来自眼动的证据. 心理学报, 42(12),1128-1136.
郭云飞, 辛聪, 干加裙, 陈幼贞. (2017). 任务规律性对前瞻记忆的影响. 心理科学, 40(6), 1309-1315.
[1] LI Wei-Jian, XIE Rui-Bo, CHEN Hai-De, HUANG Jie. The Effects of Habitual Response and Agenda-Driven on Study Time Allocation:Evidence from Eye Movement [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2014, 30(3): 277-283.
[2] TANG Zhi-wen, XING Qiang. Real and Artificial Linguistic Labels Effect in Category Learning:Evidence from Eye Movements [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2013, 29(5): 475-482.
Viewed
Full text


Abstract

Cited

  Shared   
  Discussed   
No Suggested Reading articles found!