Psychological Development and Education ›› 2020, Vol. 36 ›› Issue (2): 138-145.doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2020.02.02
Previous Articles Next Articles
XIN Cong1, ZHANG Manman1, GUO Yingxiu1, GUO Yunfei2, CHEN Youzhen1
CLC Number:
Anderson, F. T., & Einstein, G. O. (2016). The fate of completed intentions. Memory, 25(4),467-480. Boywitt, C. D., Rummel, J., & Meiser, T. (2015). Commission errors of active intentions:The roles of aging, cognitive load, and practice. Aging Neuropsychology and Cognition, 22(5),560-576. Bugg, J. M., & Scullin, M. K. (2013). Controlling intentions:The surprising ease of stopping after going relative to stopping after never having gone. Psychological science, 24(12), 2463-2471. Bugg, J. M., Scullin, M. K., & McDaniel, M. A. (2013). Strengthening encoding via implementation intention formation increases prospective memory commission errors. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 20(3),522-527. Bugg, J. M., Scullin, M. K., & Rauvola, R. S. (2016). Forgetting no-longer-relevant prospective memory intentions is (sometimes) harder with age but easier with forgetting practice. Psychology and Aging, 31(4),358-369. Dasse, M. N. (2016). Investigating prospective memory commission errors(Unpublished doctorial dissertation). Baylor University, Texas. Einstein, G. O., & McDaniel, M. A. (1990). Normal aging and prospective memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition, 16(4), 717-726. Kimmel, S. E., Chen, Z., Price, M., Parker, C. S., Metlay, J. P., & Christie, J. D. (2007). The influence of patient adherence on anticoagulation control with warfarin:Results from the international normalized ratio adherence and genetics (in-range) study. Archives of Internal Medicine, 167(3), 229-235. Meiser, T. & Rummel, J. (2012). False prospective memory responses as indications of automatic processes in the initiation of delayed intentions. Consciousness and Cognition, 21(3), 1509-1516. Pink, J. E., & Dodson, C. S. (2013). Negative prospective memory:Remembering not to perform an action. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 20(1), 184-190. Rose, F. D., Attree, E. A., Brooks, B. M., Parslow, D. M., Penn, P. R., & Ambihaipahan, N. (2000). Training in virtual environments:Transfer to real world tasks and equivalence to real task training. Ergonomics, 43(4), 494-511. Schaper, P., & Grundgeiger, T. (2016). Commission errors in delay-execute prospective memory tasks. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70(8), 1423-1438. Scott, I. A. (2016). The role of context and memory strategies in prospective memory commission errors(Unpublished doctorial dissertation). Tennessee University, Chattanooga. Scullin, M. K., & Bugg, J. M. (2013). Failing to forget:Prospective memory commission errors can result from spontaneous retrieval and impaired executive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition, 39(3), 965-977. Scullin, M. K., Bugg, J. M., & McDaniel, M. A. (2012). Whoops, I did it again:Commission errors in prospective memory. Psychology and Aging, 27(1),46-53. Scullin, M. K., Bugg, J. M., McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. (2011). Prospective memory and aging:Preserved spontaneous retrieval, but impaired deactivation, in older adults. Memory and Cognition, 39(7),1232-1240. Scullin, M. K., Einstein, G. O., & McDaniel, M. A. (2009). Evidence for spontaneous retrieval of suspended but not finished prospective memories. Memory and Cognition, 37(4), 425-433. Van Benthem, K. D., Herdman, C. M., Tolton, R. G., & Lefevre, J. A. (2015). Prospective memory failures in aviation:Effects of cue salience, workload, and individual differences. Aerospace Medicine and Human Performance, 84(4), 366-373. Walser, M., Fischer, R., & Goschke, T. (2012). The failure of deactivating intentions:Aftereffects of completed intentions in the repeated prospective memory cue paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38(4),1030-1044. Walser, M., Goschke, T., & Fischer, R. (2014). The difficulty of letting go:Moderators of the deactivation of completed intentions. Psychological Research, 78(4), 574-583. Walser, M., Goschke, T., Möschl, M., & Fischer, R. (2016). Intention deactivation:Effects of prospective memory task similarity on aftereffects of completed intentions. Psychological Research, 81(5), 961-981. West, R., Mcnerney, M. W., & Travers, S. (2007). Gone but not forgotten:The effects of cancelled intentions on the neural correlates of prospective memory. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 64(3), 215-225. Willis, M. (2016). Intention Interference, Intention Superiority and Commission Errors in Prospective Memory Tasks after Suspended or Finished Instructions(Unpublished doctorial dissertation). Baylor University, Texas. 陈丽君, 郑雪. (2014). 大学生问题发现过程的眼动研究. 心理学报, 46(3),367-384. 陈思佚, 周仁来. (2010). 前瞻记忆需要经过策略加工:来自眼动的证据. 心理学报, 42(12),1128-1136. 郭云飞, 辛聪, 干加裙, 陈幼贞. (2017). 任务规律性对前瞻记忆的影响. 心理科学, 40(6), 1309-1315. |
[1] | LI Wei-Jian, XIE Rui-Bo, CHEN Hai-De, HUANG Jie. The Effects of Habitual Response and Agenda-Driven on Study Time Allocation:Evidence from Eye Movement [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2014, 30(3): 277-283. |
[2] | TANG Zhi-wen, XING Qiang. Real and Artificial Linguistic Labels Effect in Category Learning:Evidence from Eye Movements [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2013, 29(5): 475-482. |
|