Psychological Development and Education ›› 2023, Vol. 39 ›› Issue (5): 617-624.doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2023.05.02

Previous Articles     Next Articles

Perceptual Processing Effect of the Chinese Single Character for College Students

HU Fawen1, LI Yingbing1, LI Liju2   

  1. 1. College of Teacher Education, Honghe University, Yunnan Mengzi 661199;
    2. School of Mathematics and Statistics, Honghe University, Yunnan Mengzi 661199
  • Published:2023-09-11
  • Supported by:
    The project was supported by the National Key Research and Development Program of China (2019YFC1905301);National Natural Science Foundation of China (22078115,21776108,21690083,22008078).

Abstract: The present study investigated the perceptual precedence of Chinese single characters by using the recognition paradigm of Navon-type compound stimuli in college students. The results indicated that at the configural relationship level, the recognition of a single character appeared a significant stroke number effect, and the prior processing of local feature, which was influenced by the practice and cognitive chunking effects. But at the orientating relationship the recognition of compound single characters presented the global processing precedence, and was slower than both the recognition of monomer characters and compound geometrical shapes, which were not affected by both stroke number and local features. However, when the compound single characters' local components loaded certain independent semantic or configural meanings, this global precedence effect was reversed, while the local precedence was enhanced. In short, the pattern of visual perception mainly depended on the local and global configural precedence, as well as the local semantic information. Hence, the conjunction with global and local processing facilitated the recognition of the Chinese single characters.

Key words: single characters, word recognition, global precedence, perceptual processing

CLC Number: 

  • B844
Braisby, N., & Angus, G. (2005). Cognitive psychology. New York: Oxford University Press.
Collins, A. M., & Quillian, M. R. (1969). Retrieval time from semantic memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 8(2), 240-247.
Han, S., Humphreys, G. W., & Chen, L. (1999). Parallel and competitive processes in hierarchical analysis: Perceptual grouping and encoding of closure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25(5), 1411-1432.
Kinchla, R. A., & Wolfe, J. M. (1979). The order of visual processing: “Top-down”, “bottom-up”, or “middle-out”. Perception & Psychophysics, 25(2), 225-231.
Lasaga, M. I. (1989). Gestalts and their components: Nature of information-precedence. In S. Bryan,& E. S. Ballesteros(Eds.), Object perception: Structure and process(pp. 165-202). Hillsdale,NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Marr, D. (1982). Vision: A computational investigation into the human representation and processing of visual information. New York: Henry Holt and Co.
Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception. Cognitive Psychology, 9(3), 353-383.
Perfect, T. J., Weston, N. J., Dennis, I., & Snell, A. (2008). The effects of precedence on Navon-induced processing bias in face recognition. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61(10), 1479-1486.
Plaisted, K., Swettenham, J., & Rees, L. (1999). Children with autism show local precedence in a divided attention task and global precedence in a selective attention task. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines, 40(5), 733-742.
Taft, M., & Zhu, X. (1977). Submorphemic processing in reading Chinese. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory & Cognition, 23(3), 761-775.
Treisman, A. M., & Gelade, G. (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12(1), 97-136.
包广华, 黄民盼, 李怡莲, 陈柳君, 梁雨青. (2015). 复合汉字刺激心理旋转加工中的整体优先效应. 心理技术与应用, (4), 9-14.
常玉林, 王丹烁, 周蔚. (2016). 字形判断过程中的整体与局部优先效应: 来自反应时和眼动指标的证据. 心理科学, 39(5), 1040-1044.
陈兰, 翟细春, 周新林. (2009). 两位数的整体与局部加工. 心理学报, 41(5), 406-413.
丁锦红, 张钦, 郭春彦, 魏萍. (2014). 认知心理学(第2版). 中国人民大学出版社.
杜平, 张素文, 吴颖谦, 王天珍. (1999). 汉字视知觉的整体或局部优先效应的实验研究. 武汉汽车工业大学学报, (4), 23-26.
郭小朝. (2000). 汉字识别早期知觉过程中的整体优先效应. 心理科学, 23(5), 576-580.
韩世辉. (2000). 视觉信息加工中的整体优先性. 心理学报, 32(5), 337-347.
何华, 俞华华, 刘建平. (2015). 整体-局部条件下的数字加工研究. 心理学探新, 35(6), 499-501.
孔令跃, 张豹. (2020). 汉字亚词汇部件语音加工的P200效应. 心理与行为研究, 18(3), 304-310.
李寿欣, 蔺佳, 孙华. (2015). 不同认知方式个体在知觉和心理旋转加工过程中的整体优先效应. 苏州大学学报(教育科学版), 3(5), 76-84.
梁宁建. (2006). 心理学导论.上海教育出版社.
刘明慧, 张明, 隋洁. (2014). 自我信息对知觉选择中整体优先性的调控作用. 心理学报, 46(3), 312-320.
罗春荣, 纪桂萍, 房路江. (1987). 从汉字识别看视觉信息检测的整体优先性. 心理学报, (2), 184-189.
马利军, 韩镇洋. (2019). 整字-部件关系对等级汉字识别的影响研究. 心理与行为研究, 17(6), 756-764.
彭聃龄, 王春茂. (1997). 汉字加工的基本单元:来自笔画数效应和部件数效应的证据. 心理学报, (1), 9-17.
邱香, 傅小兰, 隋丹妮, 李健, 唐一源. (2009). 复合字母刺激心理旋转加工中的整体优先效应. 心理学报, 41(1), 1-9.
余成武, 张敏. (2014). 汉字单字识别的正字法加工方式. 杭州师范大学学报(自然科学版), 13(1), 29-34.
张积家. (1977). 知觉整体优先性的研究及其进展. 应用心理学, 3(1), 52-57.
张积家. (2007). 整体与部分的意义关系对汉字知觉的影响. 心理科学, (5), 1095-1098.
张玉静. (2013). 不同趋近动机强度的积极情绪对心理旋转整体优先效应的影响 (硕士学位论文). 首都师范大学,北京.
[1] YUAN Juanjuan, YANG Yang, ZHENG Zhiwei, LIU Pingping. Picture-text Horizontal Layout Enhances Recognition of Unfamiliar Words: Evidence from 8-year-old Children and Adults [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2021, 37(4): 525-538.
Viewed
Full text


Abstract

Cited

  Shared   
  Discussed   
No Suggested Reading articles found!