Psychological Development and Education ›› 2020, Vol. 36 ›› Issue (3): 257-264.doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2020.03.01

    Next Articles

The Influence of Surface Similarity on the Understanding of Relational Similarity

HAN Congcong1, CHEN Yinghe2, YU Xiao2, DENG Zhijun3, LIU Jing4, HOU Jiangwen5, LIN Yanyan6   

  1. 1. School of Psychology, Shandong Normal University, Jinan, 250358;
    2. School of Developmental Psychology, Faculty of Psychology, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, 100875;
    3. School of Child Development and Education, China Women's University, Beijing, 100101;
    4. Weihai Wendeng Longgang Experimental Kindergarten, Weihai, 264400;
    5. Weihai Wengdeng Dazhong primary school, Weihai, 264400;
    6. Weihai Wengdeng Wenfeng primary school, Weihai, 264400
  • Published:2020-06-02

Abstract: The present study used numerical matching task and relative size matching task, both of which include high surface similarity condition, low surface similarity condition and crossmapping condition, to investigate the influence of surface similarity on children and adults' understanding of relational similarity. In experiment 1, 37 children were tested at three points in a one-year longitudinal study. The results revealed that children's relational understanding improved from age four to age five. Surface similarity influenced 4-year-olds' numerical relational understanding and relative size understanding of 4-year-olds and 5-year-olds. In experiment 2, 44 adults completed the same two matching tasks on computer. The results showed that surface similarity influenced adults' relational understanding.

Key words: surface similarity, crossmapping effect, longitudinal study

CLC Number: 

  • B844
Aïte, A., Berthoz, A., Vidal, J., Roëll, M., Zaoui, M., & Houdé, O., et al. (2016). Taking a third-person perspective requires inhibitory control:evidence from a developmental negative priming study. Child Development, 87(6), 1825-1840.
Blanchette, I., & Dunbar, K. (2000). How analogies are generated:the roles of structural and superficial similarity. Memory & Cognition, 28(1), 108-124.
Borst, G., Poirel, N., Pineau, A., Cassotti, M., & Houdé, O. (2013). Inhibitory control efficiency in a piaget-like class-inclusion task in school-age children and adults:a developmental negative priming study. Developmental Psychology, 49(7), 1366-1374.
Carlson, S. M. (2005). Developmentally sensitive measures of executive function in preschool children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28(2), 595-616.
Catrambone, R. (2002). The effects of surface and structural feature matches on the access of story analogs. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28(2), 318-334.
Clearfield, M. W., & Mix, K. S. (2001). Amount versus number:infants' use of area and contour length to discriminate small sets. Journal of Cognition and Development, 2(3), 243-260.
Daurignac, E., Houdé, O., & Jouvent, R. (2006). Negative priming in a numerical piaget-like task as evidenced by ERP. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(5), 730-736.
Deng, W., & Sloutsky, V. M. (2016). Selective attention, diffused attention, and the development of categorization. Cognitive Psychology, 91, 24-62.
Dumas, D. (2017). Relational reasoning in science, medicine, and engineering. Educational Psychology Review, 29(1), 73-95.
Feigenson, L., Carey, S., & Spelke, E.S.(2002). Infants' discrimination of number vs. continuous extent. Cognitive Psychology, 44(1), 33-66.
Gelman, R. (1998). Domain specificity in cognitive development:Universals and nouniversals. IN Sabourin, M., Craik, F. and Robert, M. (Eds) Advances in psychological science. Psychology Press Ltd., Vol 2, (25), 557-579.
Gentner, D., & Medina, J. (1998). Similarity and the development of rules. Cognition, 65(2-3), 263-297.
Gentner, D. (1988). Metaphor as structure mapping:the relational shift. Child Development, 59(1), 47-59.
Ghetti, S., & Lee, J. (2011). Children's episodic memory. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews Cognitive Science, 2(4), 365-373.
Grossnickle, E. M., Dumas, D., Alexander, P. A., & Baggetta, P. (2016). Individual differences in the process of relational reasoning. Learning & Instruction, 42, 141-159.
Hanania, R., & Smith, L. B. (2010). Selective attention and attention switching:towards a unified developmental approach. Developmental Science, 13(4), 622-635.
Haryu, E., Imai, M., & Okada, H. (2011). Object similarity bootstraps young children to action-based verb extension. Child Development, 82(2), 674-686.
Houdé, O., Pineau, A., Leroux, G., Poirel, N., Perchey, G., Lanoe, C., et al. (2011). Functional MRI study of Piaget's conservation-of-number task in preschool and school-age children:a neo-Piagetian approach. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 110, 332-346.
Landau, B., Smith, L., & Jones, S. (1998). Object shape, object function, and object name. Journal of Memory & Language, 38(1), 1-27.
Lubin, A., Simon, G., Houdé, O., & Neys, W. D. (2015). Inhibition, conflict detection, and number conservation. International Journal on mathematics education, 47(5), 793-800.
Markman, A. B., & Ross, B. H. (2003). Category use and category learning. Psychological Bulletin, 129(4), 592-613.
Mix, K. S., Huttenlocher, J., & Levine, S. C. (2002). Multiple cues for quantification in infancy:is number one of them? Psychological Bulletin, 128(2), 278-294.
Mix, K. S. (2008a). Children's numerical equivalence judgments:crossmapping effects. Cognitive Development, 23(1), 191-203.
Mix, K. S. (2008b). Surface similarity and label knowledge impact early numerical comparisons. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 26(1), 13-32.
Paik, J. H., & Mix, K. S. (2008). It's all relative:Different levels of relational similarity used in children's comparisons. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 26(4), 499-505.
Piaget, J. (1997). The child's conception of number. London:Rout-ledge. (Original Work published 1941)
Rattermann, M. J., & Gentner, D. (1998). The effect of language on similarity:The use of relational labels improves young children's performance in a mapping task. In K. Holyoak, D. Gentner, & B. Kokinov (Eds.), Advances in analogy research:Integration of theory and data from the cognitive, computational, and neural sciences (pp.274-282). Sofia:New Bulgarian University.
Richland, L. E., Morrison, R. G., & Holyoak, K. J. (2006). Children's development of analogical reasoning:insights from scene analogy problems. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 94(3), 249-273.
Sloutsky, V. M. (2003). The role of similarity in the development of categorization. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(6), 246-251.
Sloutsky, V. M. (2010). From perceptual categories to concepts:what develops? Cognitive Science, 34(7), 1244-1286.
Sloutsky, V. M. (2015). Conceptual Development. Handbook of Child Psychology and Developmental Science, 469-518
Sloutsky, V. M., Deng, W., Fisher, A. V., & Kloos, H. (2015). Conceptual influences on induction:a case for a late onset. Cognitive Psychology, 82, 1-31.
Spelke, E. S. (2000). Core knowledge. American Psychologist, 55(11), 1233-1243.
Wynn, K. (1995). Origins of numerical knowledge. Mathematical Cognition, 1, 35-60.
Yuan, L., Uttal, D., & Gentner, D. (2017). Analogical processes in children's understanding of spatial representations. Developmental Psychology, 53(6), 1098-1114.
陈英和. (2015). 儿童数量表征与数概念的发展特点及机制. 心理发展与教育, 31(1), 21-28.
韩瑽瑽, 陈蒲晶, 陈英和. (2010). 儿童数概念发展的影响因素:表面相似性效应与标签效应. 心理发展与教育, 26(5), 449-456.
李红. (2015). 中国儿童推理能力发展的初步研究. 心理与行为研究, 13(5), 637-647.
李美佳, 庄丹琪, 彭华茂. (2018). 基于问题解决式的类比推理的老化:表面相似性和结构相似性的作用.心理学报, 50(11):1282-1291.
邱琴. (2012). 基于能力的类比推理映射转换的实验研究. 心理与行为研究, 10(6), 419-426.
辛自强. (2007). 关系-表征复杂性模型. 心理发展与教育, 23(3), 122-128.
[1] DING Fangyuan, YANG Yiying, WANG Yangqian, JIA Yuncheng, CHENG Gang. Family Socioeconomic Status and College Freshmen's Attitudes towards Short-term Mating: An Analysis Based on Latent Growth Modeling [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2023, 39(5): 663-672.
[2] ZHENG Xianliang, CHEN Huiping, WANG Xue, BAO Zhenzhou. Development Trends of Adolescents’ Internet Altruistic Behavior and the Influence of Social Class: A Longitudinal Study [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2023, 39(3): 333-341.
[3] ZHANG Yuping, DONG Qiong, SONG Shuang, SHU Hua. The Developmental Trajectories of Reading Abilities in Chinese Lower Graders and the Predictability of Early Reading-related Cognitive Skills [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2023, 39(2): 210-218.
[4] ZHANG Chao, CHENG Yahua, LI Liping, FAN Yuting, WU Xinchun. The Role of Awareness of Compound Word Structures in Chinese Children's Reading Comprehension [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2022, 38(2): 236-243.
[5] ZHOU Tingna, LI Yixun, LI Hong, XU Zhonggeng, ZHANG Feng, CHENG Yahua. The Relation between Oral Reading Fluency and Reading Comprehension among Chinese Children: A 3-year Longitudinal Study [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2021, 37(5): 691-700.
[6] CHEN Zixun, FENG Yingxue, SONG Wenli, LIU Xia. The Relationship of Parent-child Separation, Peer Victimization and Depression in Adolescents: A Longitudinal Study [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2021, 37(3): 429-438.
[7] ZHENG Qiao, GENG Lina, LUO Fang, LI Lingyan. Development Trajectories of Student Engagement among Middle School Students: Associations with Peer Victimization [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2020, 36(2): 157-167.
[8] TIAN Yuan, SONG Youzhi, ZHOU Zongkui, HUANG Chun, ZHAO Yiqian. A Multilevel Analysis of The Developmental Trajectory of Belief in a Just World in Adolescence and Its Predictors [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2019, 35(2): 146-156.
[9] FANG Chenghao, CHENG Yahua, WU Xinchun. The Influence of Morphological Awareness and Oral Vocabulary Knowledge on Chinese Children's Reading Ability: A Longitudinal Study [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2019, 35(1): 57-67.
[10] CHENG Yahua, WU Xinchun. Reading Fluency of First-graders Predicted Reading Comprehension of Second-and Third-graders [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2018, 34(3): 314-321.
[11] SU Zhiqiang, WANG Gang, LIU Chuanxing, ZHANG Dajun. The Development Trend of Depression and Its Concurrency with Problem Behaviors during Middle and Late Childhood: A Two-year Longitudinal Study [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2018, 34(2): 200-209.
[12] YE Zhi, CHAI Xiaoyun, GUO Haiying, WENG Huanhuan, LIN Danhua. The Relationship Between Mobility, Educational Settings, Resilience And Loneliness Among Migrant Children:A Longitudinal Study [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2017, 33(5): 595-604.
[13] SU Zhiqiang, SHAO Jingjin, ZHANG Dajun, PU Jian. The Relation Between Friendship Quality and Depression During Middle and Late Childhood:A Longitudinal Analysis [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2017, 33(4): 449-456.
[14] ZHOU Xuelian, CHENG Yahua, LI Yixun, HAN Chunxiang, LI Hong. The Role of Oral Reading Fluency in Chinese Children's Reading Development [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2016, 32(4): 471-477.
[15] LIANG Zongbao, HU Rui, ZHANG Guangzhen, DENG Huihua, XIA Min. Bidirectional Relations between Maternal Meta-emotion Philosophy and Children's Social Adjustment among Preschoolers: A Longitudinal Study [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2016, 32(4): 394-401.
Viewed
Full text


Abstract

Cited

  Shared   
  Discussed   
No Suggested Reading articles found!