心理发展与教育 ›› 2024, Vol. 40 ›› Issue (3): 431-441.doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2024.03.14

• 理论探讨与进展 • 上一篇    下一篇

跨情境词汇学习的联结学习与假设检验之争

刘书云, 黄艳利, 石宇婧, 谢久书, 邓铸   

  1. 南京师范大学心理学院, 南京 210097
  • 发布日期:2024-05-15
  • 通讯作者: 谢久书, 邓铸 E-mail:jiusxie@outlook.com;zdeng_psy@163.com
  • 基金资助:
    江苏高校哲学社会科学研究一般项目(2019SJA0242)。

The Debate between Associative Learning and Hypothesis Testing in Cross-situational Word Learning

LIU Shuyun, HUANG Yanli, SHI Yujing, XIE Jiushu, DENG Zhu   

  1. School of Psychology, Nanjing Normal University, Nanjing 210097
  • Published:2024-05-15

摘要: 词汇学习是语言学习的基础,跨情境词汇学习又是词汇学习的主要形式。但是,跨情境词汇学习的学习机制还存在争论。目前,主要存在两种理论解释:一种是联结学习模型,认为学习者在每次学习中保留所有的词-物匹配假设,并对其进行保持和比较,最终根据共现频率选择联结最强的匹配假设;另外一种是假设检验模型,认为学习者只保留某词汇与某物体的一种匹配假设,并在之后的学习中不断验证。根据最新研究进展,该研究提出联结学习模型和假设检验模型的协同模型,尝试解决上述理论的分歧。未来研究需对跨情境词汇学习的协同模型进行系统验证。

关键词: 跨情境词汇学习, 语言学习, 学习机制, 联结学习, 假设检验

Abstract: Word learning is the foundation of language learning. Cross-situational word learning is a main form of word learning. However, the inner mechanism of cross-situational word learning is still under debate. Previous studies have proposed two competing theories on the cross-situational word learning: associative learning and hypothesis testing theories. Associative learning theory holds that learners memorize all word-referent associations and choose the most robust word-referent according to co-occurrence frequency. However, hypothesis testing theory holds that learners only memorize a single word-referent association and verify it across trials. To solve the previous debate, the present review proposes an integrated model of associative learning theory and hypothesis testing theory, which is based on cutting-edge research findings. Future studies should examine the integrated model systematically in cross-situational word learning.

Key words: cross-situational word learning, language learning, learning mechanism, associative learning, hypothesis testing

中图分类号: 

  • B844
Aussems, S., & Vogt, P. (2020). Adults use cross-situational statistics for word learning in a conservative way. IEEE Transactions on Cognitive and Developmental Systems, 12(2), 232-242.
Baronchelli, A., Ferrer-i-Cancho, R., Pastor-Satorras, R., Chater, N., & Christiansen, M. H. (2013). Networks in cognitive science.Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(7), 348-360.
Bassani, H. F., & Araujo, A. F. R. (2019). A neural network architecture for learning word-referent associations in multiple contexts.Neural Networks, 117, 249-267.
Benitez, V. L., Bulgarelli, F., Byers-Heinlein, K., Saffran, J. R., & Weiss, D. J. (2019). Statistical learning of multiple speech streams: A challenge for monolingual infants. Developmental Science, 23(2), Article e12896. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12896
Benitez, V. L., Yurovsky, D., & Smith, L. B. (2016). Competition between multiple words for a referent in cross-situational word learning. Journal of Memory and Language, 90, 31-48.
Benitez, V. L., Zettersten, M., & Wojcik, E. (2020). The temporal structure of naming events differentially affects children’s and adults’ cross-situational word learning.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 200, Article e104961. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2020.104961
Berens, S. C., Horst, J. S., & Bird, C. M. (2018). Cross-situational learning is supported by propose-but-verify hypothesis testing. Current Biology, 28(7), 1132-1136.
Bulgarelli, F., Weiss, D. J., & Dennis, N. A. (2020). Cross-situational statistical learning in younger and older adults. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 28(3), 346-366.
Escudero, P., Mulak, K. E., Fu, C. S. L., & Singh, L. (2016). Morelimitations to monolingualism: Bilinguals outperform monolinguals in implicit word learning. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, Article e1218. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01218
Gray, S., Lancaster, H., Alt, M., Hogan, T. P., Green, S., Levy, R., & Cowan, N. (2020). Thestructure of word learning in young school-age children. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 63(5), 1446-1466.
Hartley, C., Bird, L.-A., & Monaghan, P. (2020). Comparing cross-situational word learning, retention, and generalisation in children with autism and typical development.Cognition, 200, Article e104265. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104265
Hayakawa, S., Ning, S., & Marian, V. (2019). Fromklingon to colbertian: Using artificial languages to study word learning. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 23(1), 74-80.
Hendrickson, A. T., & Perfors, A. (2019). Cross-situational learning in aZipfian environment. Cognition, 189(6), 11-22.
Ibbotson, P., Lopez, D. G., & McKane, A. J. (2018). Goldilocksforgetting in cross-situational learning. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, Article e1301. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01301
Kachergis, G. (2018). Word learning: Associations or hypothesis testing? Current Biology, 28(9), R555-R557.
Kachergis, G., & Yu, C. (2018). Observing and modeling developing knowledge and uncertainty during cross-situational word learning. IEEE Transactions on Cognitive and Developmental Systems, 10(2), 227-236.
Kachergis, G., Yu, C., & Shiffrin,R. M. (2014). Cross-situational word learning is both implicit and strategic. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, Article e588. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00588
Kachergis, G., Yu, C., & Shiffrin, R. M. (2016). A bootstrapping model of frequency and context effects in word learning. Cognitive Science, 41(3), 590-622.
Khoe, Y. H., Perfors, A., & Hendrickson, A. T. (2019). Modeling individual performance in cross-situational word learning. Cognitive Science. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/4rtw9
Lavi-Rotbain, O., & Arnon, I. (2021). Visual statistical learning is facilitated inZipfian distributions. Cognition, 206, Article e104492. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104492
Lenhart, J., Lenhard, W., Vaahtoranta, E., & Suggate, S. (2020). More than words: Narrator engagement during storytelling increases children's word learning, story comprehension, and on-task behavior.Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 51(2), 338-351.
Medina, T. N., Snedeker, J., Trueswell, J. C., & Gleitman, L. R. (2011). How words can and cannot be learned by observation.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(22), 9014-9019.
Monaghan, P., & Mattock, K. (2012). Integrating constraints for learning word-referent mappings.Cognition, 123(1), 133-143.
Mulak, K. E., Vlach, H. A., & Escudero, P. (2019). Cross-situationallearning of phonologically overlapping words across degrees of ambiguity. Cognitive Science, 43(5), Article e12731. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12731
Piantadosi, S. T. (2014). Zipf's word frequency law in natural language: A critical review and future directions. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21(5), 1112-1130.
Poepsel, T. J., & Weiss, D. J. (2016). The influence of bilingualism on statistical word learning.Cognition, 152(4), 9-19.
Ralph, Y., Schneider, J. M., Abel, A., & Maguire, M. J. (2020). Using the N400 event-related potential to study word learning from context in children from low-and higher-socioeconomic status homes.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 191(5), Article e104758. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2019.104758
Rebuschat, P., Monaghan, P., & Schoetensack, C. (2021). Learning vocabulary and grammar from cross-situational statistics.Cognition, 206, Article e104475. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104475
Roembke, T., & McMurray, B. (2020). Interactions of explicit and implicit learning mechanisms in cross-situational word learning. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/3k7s8
Roembke, T. C., & McMurray, B. (2016). Observational word learning: Beyond propose-but-verify and associative bean counting.Journal of Memory and Language, 87, 105-127.
Roembke, T. C., & McMurray, B. (2021). Multiple components of statistical word learning are resource dependent: Evidence from a dual-task learning paradigm.Memory & Cognition, 49(5), 984-997.
Rooijen, R., Bekkers, E., & Junge, C. (2019). Beneficial effects of the mother’s voice on infants’ novel word learning.Infancy, 24(6), 838-856.
Schuler, K. D., Reeder, P. A., Newport, E. L., & Aslin, R. N. (2017). The effect of Zipfian frequency variations on category formation in adult artificial language learning. Language Learning and Development, 13(4), 357-374.
Smith, L., & Yu, C. (2008). Infants rapidly learn word-referent mappings via cross-situational statistics.Cognition, 106(3), 1558-1568.
Stevens, J. S., Gleitman, L. R., Trueswell, J. C., & Yang, C. (2017). Thepursuit of word meanings. Cognitive Science, 41(200), 638-676.
Suanda, S. H., & Namy, L. L. (2012). Detailedbehavioral analysis as a window into cross-situational word learning. Cognitive Science, 36(3), 545-559.
Tardif, T., & Wellman, H. M. (2000). Acquisition of mental state language in Mandarin- and Cantonese-speaking children.Developmental Psychology, 36(1), 25-43.
Tek, S., & Naigles, L. (2017). The shape bias as a word-learning principle: Lessons from and for autism spectrum disorder.Translational Issues in Psychological Science, 3(1), 94-103.
Trueswell, J. C., Medina, T. N., Hafri, A., & Gleitman, L. R. (2013). Propose but verify: Fast mapping meets cross-situational word learning.Cognitive Psychology, 66(1), 126-156.
Trehub, S. E., & Shenfield, T. (2007). Acquisition of early words from single-word and sentential contexts.Developmental Science, 10(2), 190-198.
Tsuji, S., Jincho, N., Mazuka, R., & Cristia, A. (2020). Communicative cues in the absence of a human interaction partner enhance 12-month-old infants’ word learning.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 191, Article e104740. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2019.104740
Venker, C. E. (2019). Cross-situational and ostensive word learning in children with and without autism spectrum disorder.Cognition, 183(1), 181-191.
Vlach, H. A., & DeBrock, C. A. (2019). Statistics learned are statistics forgotten: Children's retention and retrieval of cross-situational word learning.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 45(4), 700-711.
Warren, D. E., Roembke, T. C., Covington, N. V., McMurray, B., & Duff, M. C. (2020). Cross-situational statistical learning of new words despite bilateral hippocampal damage and severe amnesia.Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 13, Article e448. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2019.00448
Woodard, K., Gleitman, L. R., & Trueswell, J. C. (2016). Two- and three-year-olds track a single meaning during word learning: Evidence for propose-but-verify.Language Learning and Development, 12(3), 252-261.
Yu, C., & Smith, L. B. (2007). Rapid word learning under uncertainty via cross-situational statistics.Psychological Science, 18(5), 414-420.
Yu, C., & Smith, L. B. (2012). Modeling cross-situational word-referent learning: Prior questions. Psychological Review, 119(1), 21-39.
Yurovsky, D., & Frank, M. C. (2015). An integrative account of constraints on cross-situational learning.Cognition, 145, 53-62.
Yurovsky, D., Fricker, D. C., Yu, C., & Smith, L. B. (2014). The role of partial knowledge in statistical word iclearning.Psychonomic Bulletin Review, 21(1), 1-22.
Yurovsky, D., Smith, L. B., & Yu, C. (2013). Statistical word learning at scale:The baby's view is better. Developmental Science, 16(6), 959-966.
Zettersten, M., Wojcik, E., Benitez, V. L., & Saffran, J. (2018). The company objects keep: Linking referents together during cross-situational word learning.Journal of Memory and Language, 99, 62-73.
Zhang, X. P., & Dong, X. L. (2016). Revisiting Zipfian frequency: L2 acquisition of English prenominal past participles. The Modern Language Journal, 100(2), 404-427.
Zhang, Y., Chen, C.-H., & Yu, C. (2019). Mechanisms of cross-situational learning: Behavioral and computational evidence. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 56, 37-63.
Zhang, Y., Yurovsky, D., & Chen, Y. (2015). Statistical word learning is a continuous process: Evidence from the human simulation paradigm. Proceedings of the 37th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 2793-2798). Cognitive Science Society.
Zhang, Y., Yurovsky, D., & Yu, C. (2021). Cross-situationallearning from ambiguous egocentric input is a continuous process: Evidence using the human simulation paradigm. Cognitive Science, 45(7), Article e13010. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.13010
任婷婷, 莫雷. (2015). 数据积累,还是假设检验?——幼儿早期词汇学习机制的研究[摘要]. 见第十八届全国心理学学术会议 (pp. 1232-1233). 中国心理学会.
朱莉琪, 孟祥芝, Tardif, T. (2011). 儿童早期词汇获得的跨语言/文化研究. 心理科学进展, 19(02), 175-184.
[1] 程瑶, 杨振, 黄秀梅, 陶沙. 小学儿童英语正字法技能的发展及其在单词阅读中的作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2014, 30(1): 46-54.
[2] 郑小蓓, 王正科, 刘冬梅, 许婕, 李文玲, 孟祥芝. 语音训练对幼儿英语语音意识和字母知识的促进[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2009, 25(1): 66-71.
[3] 陶沙, 冯艳皎, 李伟. 语音意识的不同成分在汉语儿童英语阅读学习中的作用[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2007, 23(2): 82-92.
[4] 孙燕青. 重述:第二语言学习中的重要反馈方式[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2005, 21(4): 116-121.
Viewed
Full text


Abstract

Cited

  Shared   
  Discussed   
No Suggested Reading articles found!