Psychological Development and Education ›› 2015, Vol. 31 ›› Issue (2): 137-148.doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2015.02.02
Previous Articles Next Articles
ZHANG Huan1, XIN Ziqiang2
CLC Number:
Adolph, K. E., Robinson, S. R., Young, J. W., & Gill, A. F. (2008). What is the shape of developmental change? Psychological Review, 115, 527-543. Boncoddo, R., Dixon, J. A., & Kelley, E. (2010). The emergence of a novel representation from action: evidence from preschoolers. Developmental Science, 13, 370-377. Carlson, R. A., & Yaure, R. G. (1990). Practice schedules and the use of component skills in problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 484-496. Dixon, J. A., & Bangert, A. S. (2002). The prehistory of discovery: Precursors of representational change in solving gear system problems. Developmental Psychology, 38, 918-932. Dixon, J. A., & Kelley, E. (2007). Theory revision and redescription. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(2), 111-115. Goode, M. K., Geraci, L., & Roediger, H. L. (2008). Superiority of variable to repeated practice in transfer on anagram solution. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 662-666. Helsdingen, A. S., Van Gog, T., & Van Merrinboer, J. J. G. (2011a). The effects of practice schedule on learning a complex judgment task. Learning and Instruction, 21, 126-136. Helsdingen, A. S., Van Gog, T., & Van Merrinboer, J. J. G. (2011b). The effects of practice schedule and critical thinking prompts on learning and transfer of a complex judgment task. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103, 383-398. Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). Beyond modularity: A developmental perspective on cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2013).‘Microgenetics': No single method can elucidate human learning. Human Development, 56, 47-51. Kotovsky, L., & Gentner, D. (1996). Comparison and categorization in the development of relational similarity. Child Development, 67, 2797-2822. Lavelli, M., Pantoja, A., Hsu, H., Messinger, D., & Fogel, A. (2005). Using microgenetic designs to study change processes. In D. Teti (Ed.), Handbook of research methods in developmental science (pp. 40-65). Oxford, England: Blackwell. Liu, C. H., & Xin, Z. Q. (2008). Relationship between the development of pupil's representation of area-of-rectangle problem and their fluid intelligence. Studies of Psychology and Behavior, 6(03), 206-211. Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1981). The Psychology of the Child (F. Y. Wu, Trans.).Beijing: The Commercial Press. Rau, M. A., Aleven, V., & Rummel, N. (2012). Interleaved practice in multi-dimensional learning tasks: Which dimension should we interleave? Learning and Instruction, 23, 98-114. Rodríguez, P., Lago, M. O., Enesco, I., & Guerrero, S. (2012). Children's understandings of counting: Detection of errors and pseudoerrors by kindergarten and primary school children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114, 35-46. Schwartz, D. L., & Black, J. B. (1996). Shuttling between depictive models and abstract rules: Induction and fallback. Cognitive Science, 20, 457-497. Siegler, R. S. (2004). U—shaped interest in U—shaped development—and what it means. Journal of Cognition and Development, 5, 1-10. Siegler, R. S. (2006). Microgenetic analyses of learning. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Series Eds.), & D. Kuhn & R. S. Siegler (Vol. Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol 2: Cogniton, perception, and language (6th ed., pp. 464-510). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley Siegler, R. S., & Crowley, K. (1991). The microgenetic method: A direct means for studying cognitive development. American Psychologist, 46, 606-620. Thevenot, C., & Oakhill, J. (2005). The strategic use of alternative representations in arithmetic word problem solving. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology, 58, 1311-1323. Trudeau, J. J., & Dixon, J. A. (2007). Embodiment and abstraction: Actions create relational representations. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 14, 994-1000. Xin, Z. (2008). Fourth through sixth graders' representations of area-of-rectangle problems: Influences of relational complexity and cognitive holding power. The Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 142, 581-600. Xin, Z. (2003). Validation of relational-representational complexity model. Acta Psychological Sinica, 35(4), 504-513. Xin, Z. (2007). Relational-representational complexity model. Psychological Development and Education, 23(3), 122-128. Xin, Z., & Lin, C. D. (2002). The microgenetic method: Focus on cognitive change. Advances in Psychological Science, 10(2), 206-212. Xin, Z., & Zhang, L. (2009). Measuring students' representation level on arithmetic word problems: Based on the relational-representational complexity model. Psychological Development and Education, 25(1), 34-40. Xin, Z., & Zhang, L. (2006). The change of representation and it's correlates: A microgenetic study. Acta Psychological Sinica, 38, 532-542. Xin, Z., Zhang, L. Lin, C. D., & Chi, L. P. (2006). Change of Children's representational level in the context of practice. Acta Psychological Sinica, 38, 189-196. Zhang, L. (2008). Children's representation on tasks with different complexities and the relationship between representation and working memory. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Beijing Normal University. Zhang, L., Xin, Z., Lin, C., & Li, H. (2009). The complexity of the Latin Square task and its influence on children's performance. Chinese Science Bulletin, 54, 766-775. Zhang, L., Xin, Z. & Gulizha, B. (2010). The developmental characteristics of analogical reasoning on tasks with different complexity in 5-9 years old children. Psychological Development and Education, 26, 584-591. 刘春晖, 辛自强. (2008). 小学生数学问题表征发展与流体智力的关系. 心理与行为研究, 6(03), 206-211. 皮亚杰, 英海尔德. (1981). 儿童心理学(吴福元 译). 北京: 商务印书馆. 辛自强 (2003). 关系-表征复杂性模型的检验. 心理学报, 35(4), 504-513. 辛自强 (2007). 关系-表征复杂性模型. 心理发展与教育, 23(3), 122-128. 辛自强, 林崇德. (2002). 微观发生法:聚焦认知变化. 心理科学进展, 10(2), 206-212. 辛自强, 张莉. (2009). 基于关系-表征复杂性模型的数学应用题表征能力测验. 心理发展与教育, 25(1), 34-40. 辛自强, 张丽. (2006). 表征变化及其影响因素的微观发生研究. 心理学报, 38, 532-542. 辛自强, 张丽, 林崇德, 池丽萍. (2006). 练习背景下表征水平的变化. 心理学报, 38, 189-196. 张丽. (2008). 儿童对复杂性不同任务的表征及其与工作记忆的关系. 北京师范大学.博士学位论文 张莉, 辛自强, 古丽扎伯克力. (2010). 5~9岁儿童在不同复杂性任务上类比推理的发展特点. 心理发展与教育, 26, 584-591. |
[1] | ZHANG Li, XIN Zi-qiang, Gulizha, Bokeli. The Developmental Characteristics of Analogical Reasoning on Tasks with Different Complexity in 5~9 Years Old Children [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2010, 26(6): 584-591. |
[2] | XIN Zi-qiang, ZHANG Li. Measuring Students’ Representation Level on Arithmetic Word Problems:Based on the Relational-Representational Complexity Model [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2009, 25(1): 34-40,53. |
[3] | ZHANG Li, XIN Zi-qiang. Prior Analysis and Post Hoc Analysis on the Complexity of the Balance Scale Task [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2008, 24(2): 46-53. |
|