Psychological Development and Education ›› 2013, Vol. 29 ›› Issue (6): 571-577.

Previous Articles     Next Articles

Attention Processing Characteristics of Children with Autism:Evidence from the Multiple Objects Tracking Task

MA Yu1,2, ZHANG Xue-min1,3, ZHANG Ying-li1,5, WEI Liu-qing4   

  1. 1. Beijing Normal University, School of Psychology, Beijing 100875, China;
    2. Ideological and Political Education Department, Ningxia Business Technology Institute, Yinchuan 750021, China;
    3. State Key Lab of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning, Beijing 100875, China;
    4. Henan Institute of Sport Science, Zhengzhou 450044, China;
    5. Zhuhai Medical Education School, Zhuhai 519020, China
  • Online:2013-11-15 Published:2013-11-15

Abstract: The present study was intended to investigate the visual attention processing characteristics of children with autism using dynamic visual tracking task. In present study, the typical multiple objects tracking paradigm was adopted,both age and gender matched mid-low Functioning autistic children and normal control group children participated in the same tracking task。There were eight objects in the tracking task,which one, two, three, or four objects were targets and the others were non targets (distractors), the multiple objects tracking accuracy was recorded, and tracking capacity was calculated. The results showed that: (1) Tracking accuracy of autism was significantly lower than the normal control group children from one target to four targets conditions, which indicated autism group had low performance on tracking stability and persistence; (2) when the target number was one, there was no significant difference on the tracking capacity between normal control group children and autistic children; when the number of targets were two, three, and four, the tracking capacity of normal group of children were high than the autistic children significantly. This indicated that autism group was one focus attentional tracking mode and low performance on attentional shift.

Key words: Mid-low Functioning Autistic Children, Multiple Object Tracking, Characteristics of Visual Attention

CLC Number: 

  • B844.1
Ashida, H., Seiffert, A. E., & Osaka, N. (2001). Inefficient visual search for second-order motion. JOSA A, 18(9), 2255-2266.
Benson, V., Piper, J., & Fletcher-Watson, S. (2009). Atypical saccadic scanning in autistic spectrum disorder. Neuropsychologia, 47(4), 1178-1182.
Bertone, A., Mottron, L., Jelenic, P., & Faubert, J. (2003). Motion perception in autism: a complex issue. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(2), 218-225.
Dawson, G., Meltzoff, A. N., Osterling, J., Rinaldi, J., & Brown, E. (1998). Children with autism fail to orient to naturally occurring social stimuli. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 28(6), 479-485.
Dawson, G., Webb, S. J., & McPartland, J. (2005). Understanding the nature of face processing impairment in autism: insights from behavioral and electrophysiological studies. Developmental neuropsychology, 27(3), 403-424.
Drew, T., McCollough, A. W., Horowitz, T. S., & Vogel, E. K. (2009). Attentional enhancement during multiple-object tracking. Psychonomic bulletin & review, 16(2), 411-417.
Eagle, R. S. (2002). Accessing and assessing intelligence in individuals with lower functioning autism. Journal of Developmental Disabilities, 9, 45-53.
Falck-Ytter, T., Fernell, E., Hedvall, A. L., von Hofsten, C., & Gillberg, C. (2012). Gaze performance in children with autism spectrum disorder when observing communicative actions. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 42(10), 2236-2245.
Fletcher-Watson, S., Leekam, S. R., Benson, V., Frank, M. C., & Findlay, J. M. (2009). Eye-movements reveal attention to social information in autism spectrum disorder. Neuropsychologia, 47(1), 248-257.
Fombonne, & Eric.(1999). The epidemiology of autism: a review. Psychological medicine, 4(29), 769-786.
Happé, F. (1999). Autism: cognitive deficit or cognitive style? Trends in cognitive sciences, 3(6), 216-222.
Hill, E. L. (2004). Evaluating the theory of executive dysfunction in autism. Developmental Review, 24(2), 189-233.
Horowitz, T. S., Klieger, S. B., Fencsik, D. E., Yang, K. K., Alvarez, G. A., & Wolfe, J. M. (2007). Tracking unique objects. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 69(2), 172-184.
Iarocci, G., & Burack, J. A. (2004). Intact covert orienting to peripheral cues among children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(3), 257-264.
Johnson, M. H., Griffin, R., Csibra, G., Halit, H., Farroni, T., De Haan, M., et al. (2005). The emergence of the social brain network: Evidence from typical and atypical development. Development and psychopathology, 17(3), 599-619.
Jones, C. R., Swettenham, J., Charman, T., Marsden, A. J., Tregay, J., Baird, G., et al. (2011). No evidence for a fundamental visual motion processing deficit in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Autism Research, 4(5), 347-357.
Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort Engwood cliff, N.J.:Prentice-Hall.
Kikuchi, Y., Senju, A., Akechi, H., Tojo, Y., Osanai, H., & Hasegawa, T. (2011). Atypical disengagement from faces and its modulation by the control of eye fixation in children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 41(5), 629-645.
Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F., & Cohen, D. (2002). Defining and quantifying the social phenotype in autism. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159(6), 895.
Koldewyn, K., Weigelt, S., Kanwisher, N., & Jiang, Y. (2013). Multiple object tracking in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of autism and developmental disorders, 43(6), 1394-1405.
Kylli Inen, A., & Hietanen, J. K. (2004). Attention orienting by another's gaze direction in children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(3), 435-444.
Maekawa, T., Tobimatsu, S., Inada, N., Oribe, N., Onitsuka, T., Kanba, S., et al. (2011). Top-down and bottom-up visual information processing of non-social stimuli in high-functioning autism spectrum disorder. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5(1), 201-209.
Milne, E., Swettenham, J., Hansen, P., Campbell, R., Jeffries, H., & Plaisted, K. (2002). High motion coherence thresholds in children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43(2), 255-263.
Pylyshyn, Z. W., & Storm, R. W. (1988). Tracking multiple independent targets: Evidence for a parallel tracking mechanism. Spatial vision, 3(3), 179-197.
Rutherford, M. D., Richards, E. D., Moldes, V., & Sekuler, A. B. (2007). Evidence of a divided-attention advantage in autism. Cognitive neuropsychology, 24(5), 505-515.
Senju, A., Tojo, Y., Yaguchi, K., & Hasegawa, T. (2005). Deviant gaze processing in children with autism: an ERP study. Neuropsychologia, 43(9), 1297-1306.
Trick, L. M., Perl, T., & Sethi, N. (2005). Age-related differences in multiple-object tracking. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 60(2), 102.
Trick, L., Hollinsworth, H., & Brodeur, D. A. (2009). 3 Multiple-object tracking across the lifespan: do different factors contribute to diminished performance in different age groups? Computation, cognition, and Pylyshyn, 79.
Van der Geest, J. N., Kemner, C., Verbaten, M. N., & Van Engeland, H. (2002). Gaze behavior of children with pervasive developmental disorder toward human faces: a fixation time study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43(5), 669-678.
ZHang, X., Yan, M., & Liao, Y. (2009). Differential performance of chinese volleyball athletes and nonathletes on a multiple-object tracking task 1, 2.Perceptual and Motor Skills, 109(3), 747-756.
中华医学会精神病学分会. (2001). 中国精神障碍分类与诊断标准 第三版. 山东科学出版社.
[1] SU Jing, DUAN Dongyuan, ZHANG Xuemin. The Effect of Negative Emotion Stimulus on College Students' Performance of Multiple Object Tracking [J]. Psychological Development and Education, 2016, 32(5): 521-531.
Viewed
Full text


Abstract

Cited

  Shared   
  Discussed   
No Suggested Reading articles found!