心理发展与教育 ›› 2025, Vol. 41 ›› Issue (1): 145-152.doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2025.01.16

• 理论探讨与进展 • 上一篇    

“说出即相信”效应:基于共享现实的解释

崔梓芊1, 张环1,2,3, 刘希平1,2,3   

  1. 1. 天津师范大学心理学部, 天津 300387;
    2. 教育部人文社会科学重点研究基地天津师范大学心理与行为研究院, 天津 300387;
    3. 学生心理发展与学习天津市高校社会科学实验室, 天津 300387
  • 发布日期:2025-01-22
  • 通讯作者: 张环 E-mail:zhanghuan@tjnu.edu.cn

The “Saying-Is-Believing” Effect: From the Perspective of Shared Reality Theory

CUI Ziqian1, ZHANG Huan1,2,3, LIU Xiping1,2,3   

  1. 1. Faculty of Psychology, Tianjin Normal University, Tianjin 300387;
    2. Key Research Base of Humanities and Social Sciences of the Ministry of Education, Academy of Psychology and Behavior, Tianjin Normal University, Tianjin 300387;
    3. Tianjin Social Science Laboratory of Students' Mental Development and Learning, Tianjin 300387
  • Published:2025-01-22

摘要: 在日常对话中,发言者向听众传达信息时通常会偏向于听众的态度,且发言者随后的记忆也会发生改变,这就是“说出即相信”(Saying-Is-Believing, SIB)效应。前人研究曾通过基本信息过程和认知失调来解释这一效应,但是近期的共享现实理论提出,由于认知动机和关系动机驱动着发言者与听众创建共享现实,在交流对话的过程中,发言者根据共享现实会即时调整自己的输出信息,同时这种调整也会影响发言者的长时记忆,产生SIB效应。未来研究应聚焦于共享现实理论解释SIB效应中仍存在的困难与挑战,利用社会认知神经科学的手段提高SIB研究的外部效度,同时开展本土SIB研究并探索其在应用领域发挥的作用,构建更加系统完善的共享现实理论体系。

关键词: “说出即相信”效应, 共享现实, 认知动机, 关系动机

Abstract: In the process of interpersonal communication, speakers usually adjust their oral output according to the characteristics of the audience, which is called the audience-tuning effect. Meanwhile, speakers’ memory of the output information was changed in a long-term period, which is called the “Saying-Is-Believing” (SIB) effect. Early theories (the basic information process and the cognitive dissonance) had attempted to explain this phenomenon, but the research evidence remained controversial. According to the recent shared reality theory, humans are driven by epistemic motivation and affiliative motivation to work with others to build a shared reality representation of the world, which leads to the SIB effect. Future studies should explore the challenges and revelations of shared reality theory in explaining the SIB effect. In addition, researchers should also combine the methods of social cognitive neuroscience to explore the neural process and its cognitive explanation of the SIB effect on a broader scale and pay more attention to the widespread application of the SIB effect in real life.

Key words: “saying-is-believing” effect, shared reality, epistemic motivation, affiliative motivation

中图分类号: 

  • B844
Bevilacqua, D., Davidesco, I., Wan, L., Chaloner, K., Rowland, J., Ding, M. Z., … Dikker, S. (2019). Brain-to-brain synchrony and learning outcomes vary by student-teache dynamics: Evidence from a real-world classroom electroencephalography study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 31(3), 401-411.
Clark, A. E., & Kashima, Y. (2007). Stereotypes help people connect with others in the community: A situated functional analysis of the stereotype consistency bias in communication.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(6), 1028-1039.
Conley, T. D., Rabinowitz, J. L., & Matsick, J. L. (2016). U.S. ethnic minorities’ attitudes towardsWhites: The role of shared reality theory in intergroup relations. European Journal of Social Psychology, 46(1), 13-25.
Echterhoff, G., & Higgins, E. T. (2019).Shared reality: motivated connection and motivated cognition. In Echterhoff, G., & Higgins, E. T. (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (3rd ed.). New York: Guilford.
Echterhoff, G., Higgins, E. T., & Groll, S. (2005). Audience-tuning effects on memory: The role of shared reality.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89(3), 257-276.
Echterhoff, G., Higgins, E. T., Kopietz, R., & Groll, S. (2008). How communication goals determine when audience tuning biases memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137(1), 3-21.
Echterhoff, G., Higgins, E. T., & Levine, J. M. (2009). Shared reality: Experiencing commonality with others’ inner states about the world.Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4(5), 496-521.
Echterhoff, G., Kopietz, R., & Higgins, E. T. (2013).Adjusting shared reality: Communicators’ memory changes as their connection with their audience changes. Social Cognition, 31(2), 162-186.
Echterhoff, G., Kopietz, R., & Higgins, E. T. (2017). Shared reality in intergroup communication: Increasing the epistemic authority of an out-group audience. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 146(6), 806-825.
Hardin, C. D., & Conley, T. D. (2001). A relational approach to cognition: Shared experience and relationship affirmation in social cognition. In G.B. Moskowitz (Ed.), Cognitive social psychology: The Princeton Symposium on the legacy and future of social cognition (pp. 3-17). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Hardin, C. D., & Higgins, E. T. (1996). Shared reality: How social verification makes the subjective objective. In R. M. Sorrentino & E.T. Higgins (Eds.),Handbook of motivation and cognition: The interpersonal context (Vol. 3, pp. 28-84). New York: Guilford.
Higgins, E. T., & McCann, C. D. (1984). Social encoding and subsequent attitudes, impressions, and memory: ‘context-driven’ and motivational aspects of processing.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 26-39.
Higgins, E. T., & Rholes, W. S. (1978). “Saying is believing”: Effects of message modification on memory and liking for the person described.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 14(4), 363-378.
Hirsch, J., Zhang, X., Noah, J. A., & Ono, Y. (2018). Frontal temporal and parietal systems synchronize within and across brains during live eye-to-eye contact.Neuroimage, 157(15), 314-330.
Holper, L., Goldin, A. P., Shalom, D. E., Battro, A. M., Wolf, M., & Sigman, M. (2013). The teaching and the learning brain: A cortical hemodynamic marker of teacher-student interactions in the Socratic dialog.International Journal of Educational Research, 59, 1-10.
Kinreich, S., Djalovski, A., Kraus, L., Louzoun, Y., & Feldman, R. (2017). Brain-to-brain synchrony during naturalistic social interactions.Scientific Reports, 7, 17060. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-17339-5
Knausenberger, J., Wagner, U., Higgins, E. T., & Echterhoff, G. (2019). Epistemic authority in communication effects on memory: Creating shared reality with experts on the topic.Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 8(4), 439-449.
Kopietz, R., Hellmann, J. H., Higgins, E. T., & Echterhoff, G. (2010). Shared-reality effects on memory: Communicating to fulfill epistemic needs. Social Cognition, 28(3), 353-378.
Kruglanski, A. W., Orehek, E., Dechsne, M., & Pierro, A. (2010). Lay epistemic theory: The motivational, cognitive, and social aspects of knowledge formation.Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 4, 939-950.
Li, Z. R., Li, J. W., Hong, B., Nolte, G., Engel, A. K., & Zhang, D. (2021). Speaker-listener neural coupling reveals an adaptive mechanism for speech comprehension in a noisy environment.Cerebral Cortex, 31(10), 4719-4729.
Liang, T., Lin, Z., & Souma, T. (2021). How group perception affects what people share and how people feel: The role of entitativity and epistemic trust in the “Saying-Is-Believing” effect.Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 772864. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.728 864
Liu, L. F., Zhang Y. X., Zhou, Q., Garrett, D.D., Lu, C. M., Chan, A., … Ding, G. (2019).Auditory-articulatory neural alignment between listener and speaker during verbal communication. Cerebral Cortex, 30(3), 942-951.
Lun, J., Sinclair, S., Whitchurch, E. R., & Glenn, C. (2007). (Why) do I think what you think? Epistemic social tuning and implicit prejudice.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 957-972.
Mata, A., & Semin, G. R. (2020). Multiple shared realities: The context sensitivity of the saying-is-believing effect.Social Cognition, 39(4), 354-366.
Müller, V., & Lindenberger, U. (2014). Hyper-Brain networks support romantic kissing in humans.PLoS ONE, 9(11), e112080. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0112080
Pan, Y. F., Cheng, X. J., Zhang, Z. X., Li, X. C., & Hu, Y. (2017).Cooperation in lovers: An fNIRS-based hyperscanning study. Human Brain Mapping, 38, 831-841.
Pan, Y., Novembre, G., & Olsson, A. (2021). The interpersonal neuroscience of social learning.Perspectives on Psychological Science. 17(3), 680-695.
Parkinson, C., Kleinbaum, A. M., & Wheatley, T. (2018).Similar neural responses predict friendship. Nature Communications, 9, 332. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-017-02722-7
Pierucci, S., Echterhoff, G., Marchal, C., & Klein, O. (2014). Creating shared reality about ambiguous sexual harassment: The role of stimulus ambiguity in audience-tuning effects on memory.Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 3(4), 300-306.
Pierucci, S., Klein, O., & Carnaghi, A. (2013). You are the one I want to communicate with! Relational motives driving audience-tuning effects on memory.Social Psychology, 44, 16-25.
Rossignac-Milon, M., Bolger, N., Zee, K. S., Boothby, E. J., & Higgins, E. T. (2021). Merged minds: Generalized shared reality in dyadic relationships.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 120(4), 882-911.
Rossignac-Milon, M., & Higgins, E. T. (2018a). Epistemic companions: Shared reality development in close relationships. Current Opinion in Psychology, 23, 66-71.
Rossignac-Milon, M., & Higgins, E. T.(2018b). Beyond intrapersonal cognitive consistency: Shared reality and the interpersonal motivation for truth. Psychological Inquiry, 29(2), 86-93.
Schilbach, L., Timmermans, B., Reddy, V., Costall, A., Bente, G., Schlicht, T., & Vogeley, K. (2013).Toward a second-person neuroscience. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 36(4), 393-414.
Schmalbach, B., Hennemuth, L., & Echterhoff, G. (2019).A tool for assessing the experience of shared reality: Validation of the German SR-T. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 832. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00832
Semin, G. R. (2018). Audience tuning effects in the context of situated and embodied processes.Current Opinion in Psychology, 23, 113-116.
Shteynberg, G. (2015). Shared attention.Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(5), 579-590.
Stanley, M. L., & De Brigard, F. (2019). Moral memories and the belief in the good self.Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28(4), 387-391.
Todorov, A. (2002). Communication effects on memory and judgment.European Journal of Social Psychology, 32(April), 531-546.
Ye, J., Zhao, L., Huang, Z., & Meng, F. (2021). The audience-tuning effect of negative stereotypes in communication.Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 663814. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.663814
Zheng, L. F., Chen, C. S., Liu, W. D., Long, Y. H., Zhao, H., Bai, X. L., … Lu, C. M. (2018). Enhancement of teaching outcome through neural prediction of the students' knowledge state.Human Brain Mapping, 39(7), 3046-3057.
曹胜强. (2019). 跨文化交际中的中西方人际关系认知差异. 现代交际, 11, 55-56.
丁莹, 郑全全. (2011). “说出即相信”效应的影响因素和理论解释. 心理科学进展, 19(12), 1851-1858.
王修欣, 申怡凡. (2022). 何以应对道德自我威胁?道德记忆偏差的视角. 心理科学进展, 30(7), 1604-1611.
杨德锋, 李清, 赵平. (2014). 分享中社会联结、假想观众对品牌至爱的影响——他人回应分歧性的调节作用.心理学报, 46(07), 1000-1013.
[1] 李越, 辛自强, 兰艺华. 亲关系动机对家庭消费决策及婚姻满意度的影响[J]. 心理发展与教育, 2023, 39(3): 350-359.
Viewed
Full text


Abstract

Cited

  Shared   
  Discussed   
No Suggested Reading articles found!